Skip to main content

Examining specific and non-specific symptoms of the best-fitting posttraumatic stress disorder model in conflict-exposed adolescents

Abstract

Background

The 5th revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) construes PTSD symptoms into 4 clusters (intrusion, avoidance, negative alterations in cognitions and mood, alterations in arousal and reactivity; Model 1). However, recent literature has shown that this symptom structure does not best represent PTSD. Unfortunately, the findings of studies investigating the proposed alternative models are from consensus. Adding to the complexity of the issue of symptom-grouping models is the identification of specific and non-specific symptoms of PTSD. The present study aims to address these gaps by identifying the best-fitting PTSD model and subsequently examining what symptoms are considered specific and non-specific to PTSD in adolescent-survivors of armed political conflict and violence.

Methods

The study utilized a sample of 641 adolescent victim survivors. We conducted CFA analyses and compared nested models through the scaled χ2 difference test, while comparison of non-nested models was done using the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). The best-fitted model was used in the consequent analysis, where we statistically controlled for the effect of non-specific psychological distress on PTSD by comparing the factor loadings and factor correlations before and after accounting for distress using the Aroian z-test.

Results

The results provide support for the 7-factor hybrid model of PTSD over other proposed models for the current sample. Moreover, the data reveal that only 7 items could be construed as core symptoms, while the rest of the symptoms can be considered non-PTSD specific.

Conclusions

Overall, the findings provide support for the validity of the hybrid PTSD model among political conflict-exposed adolescents. The results also show that the DSM-5 PTSD has both specific and non-specific features in the present sample of conflict-exposed adolescents. This has potential implications for theory, practice, and treatment of the disorder.

Peer Review reports

Background

Several significant changes in the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom criteria have been introduced in the 5th revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [1]. These changes include the addition (e.g., “reckless/self-destructive behavior”), removal (e.g., “sense of foreshortened future”), and rephrasing (e.g., from “irritability or outbursts of anger” to “irritable behavior and angry outbursts”) of PTSD symptoms. Presently, DSM-5 categorizes PTSD symptoms into intrusion, avoidance, negative alterations in cognition and mood, and alterations of arousal and reactivity symptoms. While the changes in the DSM-5 PTSD addressed the criticisms in previous revisions of the diagnostic criteria, support for the DSM-5 model has been minimal compared to other proposed models [e.g., 24]. Adding to the complexity of the issue of symptom-grouping models is the identification of specific and non-specific symptoms of PTSD. These issues have implications for PTSD research and treatment. For instance, empirical support can guide future PTSD research to focus on its salient symptom clusters. Concurrently, information on its specific and non-specific symptoms can inform targeted interventions. Thus, the present study attempts to resolve these concerns by identifying the best-fitting model among existing PTSD models and subsequently examining which symptoms are possibly specific to PTSD or common to other disorders.

More differentiated models have consistently demonstrated superiority over the DSM-5 model (see Table 1). The dysphoria model (Model 2) [5] identifies a unique dysphoria (i.e., general psychological distress) factor which encompasses the DSM-5 model’s negative alterations in cognition and mood and the alterations in arousal and reactivity factors. The dysphoric arousal model (Model 3) [2] divides DSM-5’s alterations in arousal and reactivity factor into dysphoric and anxious arousal factors to distinguish between arousal symptoms that have mixed anxiety and depression features (i.e., dysphoric arousal) and those that are fear-based and more specific to anxiety disorders (i.e., anxious arousal). Building on the dysphoric arousal model, the anhedonia model (Model 4) [3] further distinguishes between negative and positive affect symptoms. This is based on the contention that these two constructs are distinct emotions and warrant separate attention in research and practice [6, 7]. Conversely, the externalizing behaviors model (Model 5) [8] retains the DSM-5’s negative alterations in cognitions and mood factor while highlighting a subset of dysphoric arousal symptoms. It identifies two symptoms of externalized, self-initiated, aggressive behaviors of individuals with PTSD called ‘externalizing behaviors’. Lastly, the hybrid model (Model 6) [4] incorporates the salient features of all previous models, resulting in seven differentiated PTSD symptom clusters (i.e., intrusion, avoidance, negative affect, anhedonia, externalizing behavior, anxious arousal, dysphoric arousal). A summary of the similarities and differences in item grouping for all the models is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Symptom mappings across competing PTSD models

A second issue emerging from the changes in DSM-5 PTSD symptomatology pertains to whether the symptoms are disorder-specific or non-specific. Several studies have found that PTSD contains several non-specific symptoms [7, 9] representing general distress [10]. These non-specific distress symptoms appear to be common across mental health disorders [see 5, 7, 11]. However, information pertaining to which specific symptoms or symptom clusters are more related to PTSD than general distress remains scant. Identifying disorder-specific and non-specific symptoms of PTSD has important implications. First, identifying non-specific symptoms of PTSD (e.g., sleep disturbance, difficulty concentrating) is important for understanding patients’ clinical presentation, degree of impairment, and probable response to different treatment modalities. Numerous studies have shown that the presence of comorbid non-specific disorder symptoms is associated with more severe PTSD and worse functionality [12, 13]. Impliedly, identifying the non-specific symptoms of PTSD can inform clinicians and mental health practitioners on the pervasiveness of traumatic events’ impact, allowing for the implementation of effective interventions [14, 15]. Second, it is equally important to identify PTSD-specific symptoms. PTSD as a diagnostic entity is presumed to have cardinal symptoms unique from other disorders. Identifying these core symptoms could lead to a more accurate understanding of PTSD and its distinctiveness from other disorders. Finally, differentiating between specific and non-specific symptoms has pragmatic treatment implications. Emerging studies show that targeting non-specific aspects can reduce overall PTSD severity [e.g., 16]. This may inform a two-pronged approach to the treatment of PTSD: the first arm can address non-specific symptoms to decrease overall symptom severity, while the second focuses on targeting disorder-specific symptoms to alleviate PTSD.

Method

Procedure and participants

The present study aims to identify the best-fitting factor structure of PTSD in the current sample and determine PTSD’s specific and non-specific symptom clusters. Prior to data gathering, we sought permission from the appropriate authorities to conduct the data gathering (i.e., division superintendent and principals of the schools). We used purposive sampling to gather data from 684 adolescents living in a province in Southern Philippines, with ‘exposure to armed political conflict and violence’ as the sole inclusion criteria. The participant’s responses on the Exposure to Conflict and Violence Scale determined exposure to trauma events and were validated through interviews with teachers and school counselors. Of the 684, we confirmed that only 641 participants have been exposed to political conflict and violence. After obtaining assent forms from the participants and informed consent forms from their parents, we invited those who met the inclusion criteria to participate in the study.

The present sample is a community sample of 641 adolescents exposed to armed political conflict and violence. All participants reside in a province in Southern Philippines. The sample is predominantly female (n = 416, 64.9%), between ages 13–17 (M = 16.47, SD = 0.89), and Muslim (n = 597, 93.1%). The frequency distribution for the exposure of trauma events is included in Supplementary Table 1. The study’s procedures adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and are reviewed and approved by an ethics committee from the Mindanao State University – Iligan Institute of Technology College of Education, Philippines.

Instruments

Exposure to political conflict and violence. Participants indicate exposure to conflict and violence on a 29-item questionnaire enumerating the most common war experiences of the people in the vicinity. The first 28 items list the event (e.g., loss of, or injury to, a friend or family member; witnessed actual violence such as seeing a stranger being arrested, injured, or killed; violence among groups of people or the population generally that took place in neighborhoods, streets, and around checkpoints that people crossed as they sought to reach work, schooling and services). One additional item allows the participant to identify experiences not included in the first 28 items. Participants indicate whether or not they experienced any of the events listed in the past year, and how often they have experienced them, using a four-point scale (0=“not at all/never” to 4=“many times/almost everyday”). The scale has a Cronbach alpha of 0.92.

Posttraumatic stress symptoms. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5) [17] is a 20-item self-report measure used to assess PTSD symptoms within the last six months. PCL-5 is based on the DSM-5 criteria of PTSD comprising four factors: intrusion (e.g., “Repeated, disturbing, and unwanted memories of the stressful experience”), avoidance (e.g., “Avoiding memories, thoughts, or feelings related to the stressful experience”), negative alterations in cognition and mood (e.g., “Having strong negative feelings such as fear, horror, anger, guilt, or shame”), and alterations in arousal and reactivity (e.g., “Being ‘super alert’ or ‘watchful or on guard’”). Participants rate each item on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“Not at all”) to 4 (“Extremely”), based on their experience of evacuating during armed conflicts. Research shows that PCL-5 is valid and reliable [e.g., 3, 18], even among Filipinos [e.g., 1921]. In the present study, the PCL-5 has a Cronbach alpha of 0.92.

Psychological distress. Psychological distress is measured using the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) [22]. The K10 consists of 10 items describing an individual’s experience of anxiety and depression symptoms within the last 30 days (e.g., “About how often did you feel tired out for no good reason”, “About how often did you feel nervous”, “About how often did you feel so nervous that nothing could calm you down”). They rate each item on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“None of the time”) to 5 (“All of the time”). Research shows the K10 to have good reliability and validity [e.g., 22, 23]. In the present study, the scale has a Cronbach alpha of 0.87.

Data analysis

Prior to any statistical analyses, we screened the data and found that values were missing completely at random (MCAR). For the K10, 40 (6.2%) participants missed 1 item, 11 (1.7%) were missing 2 items, and 47 (7.3%) were missing 3 items. For the PCL-5, 107 (16.7%) participants missed 1 item, 32 (5.0%) were missing 2 items, 7 (1.1%) were missing 3 items, 2 (0.3%) were missing 4 items, 1 (0.2%) were missing 5 items, and 28 (4.4%) were missing 7 items. We used expectation-maximization (EM) [24], as EM methods have shown effectiveness in treating missing data [62] for up to 30% missing values [25]. None of the respondents were excluded from the analysis based on missing responses. We conducted CFAs using robust maximum likelihood estimation method with mean-adjusted Satorra-Bentler chi-square (S-Bχ2) to correct for non-normality. In all CFA analyses, all factors were allowed to correlate but correlated errors were not permitted.

Comparison of nested models was done using the scaled χ2 difference test [26], while comparison of non-nested models was done using the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). Next, we created a combined PTSD-Psychological Distress model and regressed the items of the best-fitted PTSD model to the total psychological distress score, allowing us to statistically control for the effect of non-specific psychological distress on PTSD. We consequently compared the factor loadings and factor correlations before and after including psychological distress using the Aroian z-test [27]. This test helped us determine whether there is a significant change in PTSD symptoms’ factor loadings once psychological distress is controlled for.

Assessing the significant changes in PTSD symptoms’ factor loadings before and after controlling for psychological distress helped to distinguish between PTSD-specific and non-specific symptoms. This was based on the notion that when psychological distress is statistically accounted for, the factor loadings of PTSD item-symptoms will significantly decrease particularly for those with substantial associations (i.e., correlations) with psychological distress. We considered these item-symptoms that have significant shared variance with general distress as non-specific PTSD symptoms. Meanwhile, PTSD item-symptoms whose factor loadings did not significantly attenuate may have less substantial relationships with psychological distress. Given that they did not share a significant amount of variance, these symptoms were considered PTSD-specific. Several studies have utilized this approach to assess whether the symptoms were unique to PTSD or shared with other disorders [e.g., 2830]. To correct for Type I error, the Bonferroni-Holm method was used [31]. All analyses were conducted using the Mplus software version 7.11 [32].

Results

The current sample has a mean score of 26.04 (SD = 13.40) on the PCL, with participants scoring between 0 and 80. Meanwhile, the mean score for K10 is 13.05 (SD = 7.07), with participants scoring between 10 and 47. Results of the CFA are summarized in Table 2. All six models achieved adequate to excellent fit based on CFI, TLI and RMSEA values. Comparison of nested and non-nested models are summarized in Tables 2 and 3. Among the models, model 6 (hybrid model) shows the best fit to the data (S-Bχ2 (149, N = 641) = 270.061, p < 0.0001, CFI = 0.967, TLI = 0.958, RMSEA = 0.036 (C.I.=0.029–0.042)). Model 3 (dysphoric arousal model) achieves better fit than models 1 (DSM-5 model) and 2 (dysphoria model), but not models 4 (anhedonia model), 5 (externalizing behaviors model), and 6.

Table 2 Model goodness-of-fit indices (MLM)

Given these findings, we used the hybrid model in the consequent analysis. We regressed the hybrid model’s items to the observed K10 total score (see Fig. 1). The combined PTSD-Psychological Distress model also has excellent fit (S-Bχ2 (149, N = 641) = 272.016, p < 0.0001, CFI = 0.969, TLI = 0.956, RMSEA = 0.036 (C.I.=0.029–0.043)). After using the Bonferroni-Holm correction, the decrease in factor loadings for the following items are statistically significant: B2, nightmares; B5, physiological cue reactivity; C2, avoidance of reminders; D1, trauma-related amnesia; D2, negative beliefs; D3, distorted blame; D4, pervasive negative emotional state; D5, lack of interest; D6, detachment; D7, inability to experience positive emotions; E2, recklessness; E5, difficulty concentrating, and; E6, sleep disturbance. Results of this analysis are summarized in Table 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The combined PTSD-Psychological Distress model

Table 3 Factor loadings with Aroian z-test values

Discussion

The present study aims to address two objectives: first, to compare existing PTSD models, and identify the best-fitting model in a political conflict-exposed adolescent sample; and second, to examine which symptoms are PTSD-specific or non-specific. Our results indicate that the best-fitting model is the seven-factor hybrid model of PTSD [4]. The hybrid model has received particularly strong support in recent years across various samples [e.g., 20, 33]. Our results extend support for the model and contend that the hybrid model best represents the PTSD symptom structure in conflict-exposed adolescents. The hybrid model identifies seven distinct factors of PTSD: intrusion, avoidance, negative affect, anhedonia, anxious arousal, dysphoric arousal, and externalizing behaviors. A synthesis of previous models, the hybrid model incorporates three salient changes to the DSM-5 PTSD model. First, it split the negative alterations in cognition and mood factor into negative affect and anhedonia. This is based on previous theoretical and empirical evidence that positive and negative affect are distinct constructs [6]. This distinction suggests that trauma survivors’ experience of negative emotions does not necessarily imply reduced experience of positive emotions. Alternatively, the lack of positive emotions (i.e., anhedonia) does not necessarily mean they will experience negative affect (i.e., anger, guilt). Recent CFA literature supports separating the negative affect factor from the anhedonia factor [e.g., 30]. Second, the hybrid model differentiates the dysphoric arousal factor from the anxious arousal factor [2]. This is based on the notion that anxious arousal represents fear-based symptoms more specific to anxiety disorders, while dysphoric arousal represents mixed anxiety and depression indicators of arousal [2]. Differentiating dysphoric arousal and anxious arousal factors has received substantial support from previous studies [e.g., 34, 35], particularly among Asians [e.g., 36, 37] and Filipinos [e.g., 20, 30]. Lastly, the hybrid model identifies a unique externalizing behaviors factor of PTSD. Researchers contend that externalized, self-initiated aggressive behaviors are distinct from other passive internalized symptoms described by dysphoric arousal and anxious arousal factors [8, 38].

The second objective of this study is to identify which symptoms are potentially specific and non-specific. Non-specific symptoms are associated with general distress [16]. In the present study, we assumed that these symptoms are more correlated to psychological distress than PTSD. Thus, symptoms whose relationships with PTSD did not significantly decrease after controlling for psychological distress are assumed to be PTSD-specific. Alternatively, symptoms whose association with PTSD significantly decreased after accounting for distress are assumed to be non-specific to PTSD. There are several noteworthy findings. First, all item loadings in three prominent PTSD symptom groups (i.e., negative affect, anhedonia, and dysphoric arousal) consistently attenuated when psychological distress was accounted for. This suggests that they are more associated with non-specific distress than PTSD factors. These results are consistent with extant literature indicating that negative affect, anhedonia, and dysphoric arousal symptoms are also present in other mental health disorders. For instance, negative affect is also present in depression [39], generalized anxiety disorder [40], addiction problems [41], and eating disorders [42], while anhedonia is associated with anxiety [43], depression [44], and obsessive-compulsive disorder [45]. Moreover, dysphoric arousal symptoms are significantly associated with anxiety [46], depression [47], and substance use disorders [48]. This is in line with Simms and colleagues’ contention that PTSD contains a large dysphoria (i.e., general distress) component representing shared symptoms between PTSD, anxiety, and depression [5]. This dysphoria factor encompasses all three symptom groups identified as non-specific in the present study. This finding may warrant further investigation.

The second salient finding evolves around the symptoms which are not significantly accounted for by general distress. The data shows that factor loadings of anxious arousal cluster symptoms (i.e., hypervigilance, exaggerated startle response) did not significantly decrease after accounting for distress. Anxious arousal symptoms are associated with responses to a feared stimulus—a clear indication of anxiety—wherein a person with PTSD may show anxious reactions to those which remind them of a traumatic event. This in contrast to dysphoric arousal symptoms wherein people with PTSD may show not only symptoms of anxiety but also depression. Potentially, anxious arousal symptoms are unique features of PTSD while dysphoric arousal is more representative of general distress [e.g., 3, 4, 9, 49]. While more research is needed to support these assertions, our results further strengthen the argument that the negative alterations in cognition and mood cluster in DSM-5 contains separate factors of anxious arousal characterized by fear-based symptoms and dysphoric arousal factor that is comprised of anxiety- and depression-related symptoms [2].

Finally, it is theoretically and empirically interesting that PTSD symptom-clusters did not show the expected differential relationships with general distress at the symptom level. For instance, the intrusion symptom-cluster is widely considered a PTSD-specific factor [e.g., 50, 51]. However, only three (of five) symptoms in this cluster did not significantly attenuate after controlling for psychological distress. Similarly, only avoidance of thoughts (C1) from the avoidance cluster and irritability/anger (E1) from the externalizing behaviors cluster show stronger relations to PTSD after controlling for psychological distress. This suggests that PTSD’s symptom clusters are neither purely PTSD-specific nor non-specific; rather, the heterogeneity of PTSD is present up to the symptom level. This contention is relevant amid the debate on the diagnostic specificity of PTSD. The DSM-5 nomenclature adopts the notion that the wide-ranging consequences of trauma may include symptoms that PTSD shares with other disorders, while the International Classification of Diseases – 11th revision (ICD-11) supports the exclusion of shared symptoms to improve PTSD’s diagnostic specificity and parsimony [9, 52]. Previous research that has aimed to differentiate between specific and non-specific aspects of PTSD anchor their arguments on cluster-level investigations [e.g., 9, 18, 28]. However, the results suggest that such a distinction should consider not just clusters but individual symptoms as well. This does not imply the exclusion of any symptom, however, nor does it advocate for determining diagnoses as such. Simply, the results suggest that the debate regarding PTSD’s nomenclature is far from over, and that looking more closely at individual symptoms may provide a more nuanced picture of its unique properties.

Despite the present study’s contribution to the extant literature, we also recognize its limitations. First, the current sample is a community sample of adolescent-survivors of armed conflict and violence which does not demographically represent Filipino adolescents. Thus, the results are generalizable only to this subset of the population, and any interpretation outside this population must be treated cautiously. It would be interesting if future studies could assess the latent structure of PTSD in minority and marginalized groups, particularly among those who experienced oppression-related distress [53], race-based traumatic stress [54], and internal displacement adversities [21]. Since trauma type is inextricably related to PTSD severity and symptom presentation [47, 53, 55], further investigation on these topics may provide vital information in the etiology, progress, and course of PTSD. The similarities and differences in the symptom structure would substantially contribute to the trauma literature in these least studied populations. Second, this study uses self-report scales. All potential biases and pitfalls of this data-collection approach need to be noted. We also note that because we only use a single measure of psychological distress (i.e., K10), our findings may differ from those which used specific measures of psychological distress, such as anxiety, depression, or both. Further studies investigating PTSD’s unique and transdiagnostic symptoms may benefit from investigating PTSD alongside other diagnostic entities (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder) rather than general distress alone. Similarly, future research may look at the impact of other relevant variables. For example, gender appears to affect PTSD symptomatology among adolescents [e.g., 56, 57]. Though we did not find any model invariance across genders (see Supplementary Table 2), it would be interesting to investigate whether gender differences result in model invariances in PTSD, including its differential relations with psychological distress. Third, while the current findings seemingly distinguished between specific and non-specific PTSD symptoms, establishing this distinction require further research as the boundary between PTSD and other disorders is observed to be fuzzy. Fourth, while we observed significant attenuation in several PTSD symptoms after controlling for general distress, it is possible that the relationship between the symptoms may have directly or indirectly affected this decrease. This alternative possibility is beyond the scope of the present study and may be investigated in future research. Fifth, the present study recognizes the importance of utilizing a clinical sample in investigating the factor structure of PTSD. While the present study’s use of a community sample addresses a common caveat of using clinical sample—that is, the lack of generalizability in its findings, particularly to samples who have clinical subthreshold or to those who are still developing symptoms a month after the trauma event—we contend that using clinical samples may provide a more complete picture of the structure, patterns, relationships, and course development of PTSD symptoms from the onset of the trauma event to the later stages of the survivors’ responses and ways of coping. Finally, this is a cross-sectional study. Thus, we recommend conducting longitudinal studies to ascertain the stability of the hybrid model as it relates to psychological distress.

Conclusions

Amid these limitations, our study has two salient findings that provide important information to enrich the existing trauma literature. First, the findings support the validity of the hybrid model [4] over other PTSD models for the present political conflict-exposed adolescent sample. Given that the hybrid model highlights more specific symptom-factors, this has substantial implications in assessing more precise, multi-factorial PTSD symptomatology and in the development of differentiated and effective interventions targeting these more particular factors. For instance, relaxation exercises (i.e., progressive relaxation) that have been proven effective in decreasing anxiety [58] could be incorporated in lowering the symptom severity of the dysphoric arousal factor, while enhancing social interactions and physical exercise that have been shown to improve positive mood [e.g., 59, 60] could be used to reduce the symptoms of the anhedonia factor. Second, with the results showing different models to be superior to the DSM-5 PTSD model for the current sample, this study advocates for the continued efforts in refining the DSM-5 PTSD configuration and further investigating it across various samples. This may entail further investigation of newer PTSD models such as recently proposed bifactor models [e.g., 35] or revisiting older ones. For instance, our findings suggest that intrusion, avoidance, and anxious arousal symptoms appear to be most central to PTSD in the current sample. This configuration is reminiscent of the earlier three-factor model proposed by King and colleagues [61] composed of intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal. Clearly, previous PTSD models may also warrant re-examination. Our second major finding also affirms the previous literature’s observation that DSM-5 PTSD has both specific and non-specific features [e.g., 5, 9]. However, unlike past studies that identify, classify, include, or exclude non-specific symptoms based on clusters and not on specific symptoms, the current data shows that the pattern of non-specific symptoms present in PTSD is not only by clusters (i.e., negative affect, dysphoric arousal, anhedonia) but also on individual symptoms (for example, B2 and B5 symptoms of intrusion cluster are potentially non-specific). The current findings of the study, among other studies [e.g., 9, 18, 20, 28], provide additional information on the symptoms of PTSD, should DSM decide on future revisions of PTSD symptom clusters.

Data Availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BIC:

Bayesian information criterion

CFA:

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

CFI:

Comparative fit index

DSM:

5–Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th revision

EM:

Expectation Maximization

ICD:

11–International Classification of Diseases–11th revision

K10:

Kessler Psychological Distress Scale 10

NA:

Negative affect

PCL:

5–PTSD Checklist for DSM–5

PTSD:

Posttraumatic stress disorder

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

TLI:

Tucker–Lewis index

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: Author; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Elhai JD, Biehn TL, Armour C, Klopper JJ, Frueh BC, Palmieri PA. Evidence for a unique PTSD construct represented by PTSD’s D1–D3 symptoms. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25(3):340–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Liu P, Wang L, Cao C, Wang R, Zhang J, Zhang B, Wu Q, Zhang H, Zhao Z, Fan G, Elhai JD. The underlying dimensions of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in an epidemiological sample of Chinese Earthquake survivors. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(4):345–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Armour C, Tsai J, Durham TA, Charak R, Biehn TL, Elhai JD, Pietrzak RH. Dimensional structure of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress symptoms: support for a hybrid Anhedonia and Externalizing behaviors model. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;61:106–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Simms LJ, Watson D, Doebbelling BN. Confirmatory factor analyses of posttraumatic stress symptoms in deployed and nondeployed veterans of the Gulf War. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(4):637.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Watson D. Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: a quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(4):522.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Watson D. Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders: a quadripartite model. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. 2009;5(1):221–47.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Tsai J, Harpaz-Rotem I, Armour C, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, Pietrzak RH. Dimensional structure of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;76(5):4513.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Zoellner LA, Pruitt LD, Farach FJ, Jun JJ. Understanding heterogeneity in PTSD: fear, dysphoria, and distress. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(2):97–106.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Clark LA, Watson D. Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100(3):316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Mineka S, Watson D, Clark LA. Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Ann Rev Psychol. 1998;49(1):377–412.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Knowles KA, Sripada RK, Defever M, Rauch SA. Comorbid mood and anxiety disorders and severity of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in treatment-seeking veterans. Psychol Trauma: Theory Res Pract Policy. 2019;11(4):451.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Rosen V, Ortiz NF, Nemeroff CB. Double trouble: treatment considerations for patients with comorbid PTSD and depression. Curr Treat Options Psychiatry. 2020;7(3):258–74.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Galatzer-Levy IR, Ankri Y, Freedman S, Israeli-Shalev Y, Roitman P, Gilad M, Shalev AY. Early PTSD symptom trajectories: persistence, recovery, and response to treatment: results from the Jerusalem Trauma Outreach and Prevention Study (J-TOPS). PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):e70084.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Qassem T, Aly-ElGabry D, Alzarouni A, Abdel-Aziz K, Arnone D. Psychiatric co-morbidities in post-traumatic stress disorder: detailed findings from the adult psychiatric morbidity survey in the English population. Psychiatr Q. 2021;92(1):321–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Barlow DH, Allen LB, Choate ML. Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders–republished article. Behav Ther. 2016;47(6):838–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, Schnurr PP. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). Scale Available from the National Center for PTSD at www ptsd Va Gov. 2013;10(4):206.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Byllesby BM, Elhai JD, Tamburrino M, Fine TH, Cohen G, Sampson L, Shirley E, Chan PK, Liberzon I, Galea S, Calabrese JR. General distress is more important than PTSD’s cognition and mood alterations factor in accounting for PTSD and depression’s comorbidity. J Affect Disord. 2017;211:118–23.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hall BJ, Yip PS, Garabiles MR, Lao CK, Chan EW, Marx BP. Psychometric validation of the PTSD Checklist-5 among female Filipino migrant workers. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2019;10(1):1571378.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Mordeno IG, Hall BJ. DSM-5-based latent PTSD models: assessing structural relations with GAD in Filipino post-relocatees. Psychiatry Res. 2017;258:1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Mordeno IG, Nalipay MJ, Sy DJ, Luzano JG. PTSD factor structure and relationship with self-construal among internally displaced persons. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;44:102–10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SL, Walters EE, Zaslavsky AM. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32(6):959–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fassaert T, De Wit MA, Tuinebreijer WC, Wouters H, Verhoeff AP, Beekman AT, Dekker J. Psychometric properties of an interviewer-administered version of the Kessler Psychological Distress scale (K10) among Dutch, Moroccan and Turkish respondents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2009;18(3):159–68.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. Guilford press; 2010 Apr 23.

  25. Musil CM, Warner CB, Yobas PK, Jones SL. A comparison of imputation techniques for handling missing data. West J Nurs Res. 2002;24(7):815–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Satorra A, Bentler PM. A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika. 2001;66(4):507–14.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Aroian LA. The probability function of the product of two normally distributed variables. Ann Math Stat. 1947 Jun;1:265–71.

  28. Byllesby BM, Charak R, Durham TA, Wang X, Elhai JD. The underlying role of negative affect in the association between PTSD, major depressive disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2016;38(4):655–65.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Elklit A, Armour C, Shevlin M. Testing alternative factor models of PTSD and the robustness of the dysphoria factor. J Anxiety Disord. 2010;24(1):147–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Mordeno IG, Luzano JG, Mordeno ER, Ferolino MA. Investigating the latent dimensions of posttraumatic stress disorder and the role of anxiety sensitivity in combat-exposed Filipino soldiers. Military Psychol. 2020;32(3):223–36.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian J Stat 1979 Jan 1:65–70.

  32. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus 7.11. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2013 Jun.

  33. Yang H, Wang L, Cao C, Cao X, Fang R, Zhang J, Elhai JD. The underlying dimensions of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms and their relations with anxiety and depression in a sample of adolescents exposed to an Explosion Accident. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2017;8(1):1272789.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Zawadzki B, Popiel A, Foa EB, Jakubowska B, Cyniak-Cieciura M, Pragłowska E. The structure of symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder according to DSM-5 and assessed by PDS-5–preliminary results. Curr Issues Personality Psychol. 2015;3(1):1–1.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Nedelcea C, Ciorbea ID, Vasile DL, Ionescu Ș, Papasteri C, Letzner RD, Cosmoiu A, Georgescu T. The structure of PTSD. Development of the Post Traumatic Symptom scale from a clinician-based perspective. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2022;13(1):2066455.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Su YJ, Kung YW, Hung FC, Chen SH. Dimensionality of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms: validation of the Chinese version of the posttraumatic diagnostic scale for DSM-5 across multiple trauma samples. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;74:102261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wang L, Li Z, Shi Z, Zhang J, Zhang K, Liu Z, Elhai JD. Testing the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress responses in young Chinese adult Earthquake survivors: further evidence for dysphoric arousal as a unique PTSD construct. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28(12):1097–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Tiamiyu MF, Gan Y, Kwiatkowski D, Foreman KC, Dietrich A, Elliott K, Elhai JD. Relationships between latent factors of posttraumatic stress disorder and posttraumatic growth. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2016;204(5):344–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dunn BD, German RE, Khazanov G, Xu C, Hollon SD, DeRubeis RJ. Changes in positive and negative affect during pharmacological treatment and cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder: a secondary analysis of two randomized controlled trials. Clin Psychol Sci. 2020;8(1):36–51.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Ferreira-Garcia R, Faria CG, Nardi AE, da Rocha Freire RC. Negative affect mediates impulsivity in generalized anxiety disorder. Psychopathology. 2019;52(6):327–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hogarth L. Addiction is driven by excessive goal-directed drug choice under negative affect: translational critique of habit and compulsion theory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(5):720–35.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Schaefer LM, Smith KE, Anderson LM, Cao L, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Crow SJ, Peterson CB, Wonderlich SA. The role of affect in the maintenance of binge-eating disorder: evidence from an ecological momentary assessment study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2020;129(4):387.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Naragon-Gainey K. Affective models of depression and anxiety: extension to within-person processes in daily life. J Affect Disord. 2019;243:241–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Trøstheim M, Eikemo M, Meir R, Hansen I, Paul E, Kroll SL, Garland EL, Leknes S. Assessment of Anhedonia in adults with and without mental Illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Netw open. 2020;3(8):e2013233.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Pushkarskaya H, Sobowale K, Henick D, Tolin DF, Anticevic A, Pearlson GD, Levy I, Harpaz-Rotem I, Pittenger C. Contrasting contributions of anhedonia to obsessive-compulsive, hoarding, and post-traumatic stress disorders. J Psychiatr Res. 2019;109:202–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Brown WJ, Wilkerson AK, Boyd SJ, Dewey D, Mesa F, Bunnell BE. A review of sleep disturbance in children and adolescents with anxiety. J Sleep Res. 2018;27(3):e12635.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Freeman D, Sheaves B, Waite F, Harvey AG, Harrison PJ. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(7):628–37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Chakravorty S, Vandrey RG, He S, Stein MD. Sleep management among patients with substance use disorders. Med Clin. 2018;102(4):733–43.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Fried EI, Eidhof MB, Palic S, Costantini G, Huisman-van Dijk HM, Bockting CL, Engelhard I, Armour C, Nielsen AB, Karstoft KI. Replicability and generalizability of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) networks: a cross-cultural multisite study of PTSD symptoms in four trauma patient samples. Clin Psychol Sci. 2018;6(3):335–51.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. 48, Rademaker AR, van Minnen A, Ebberink F, van Zuiden M, Hagenaars MA, Geuze E. Symptom structure of PTSD: support for a hierarchical model separating core PTSD symptoms from dysphoria. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2012;3(1):17580.

    Google Scholar 

  51. 49, Walton JL, Cuccurullo LA, Raines AM, Vidaurri DN, Allan NP, Maieritsch KP, Franklin CL. Sometimes less is more: establishing the core symptoms of PTSD. J Trauma Stress. 2017;30(3):254–8.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Insel TR, Lieberman JA. DSM-5 and RDoC: shared interests.

  53. Holmes SC, Facemire VC, DaFonseca AM. Expanding criterion a for posttraumatic stress disorder: considering the deleterious impact of oppression. Traumatology. 2016;22(4):314.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Roberson K, Carter RT. The relationship between race-based traumatic stress and the Trauma Symptom Checklist: does racial trauma differ in symptom presentation? Traumatology. 2022;28(1):120.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Smith HL, Summers BJ, Dillon KH, Cougle JR. Is worst-event trauma type related to PTSD symptom presentation and associated features? J Anxiety Disord. 2016;38:55–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Armour C, Elhai JD, Layne CM, Shevlin M, Duraković-Belko E, Djapo N, Pynoos RS. Gender differences in the factor structure of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in war-exposed adolescents. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25(4):604–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hukkelberg SS. Posttraumatic stress reactions in children and adolescents: factor structure and gender invariance in the dysphoria and numbing model. Psychol Trauma: Theory Res Pract Policy. 2014;6(3):261.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Harorani M, Davodabady F, Masmouei B, Barati N. The effect of Progressive muscle relaxation on anxiety and sleep quality in burn patients: a randomized clinical trial. Burns. 2020;46(5):1107–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Kuczynski AM, Halvorson MA, Slater LR, Kanter JW. The effect of social interaction quantity and quality on depressed mood and loneliness: a daily diary study. J Social Personal Relationships. 2022;39(3):734–56.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Miller KJ, Mesagno C, McLaren S, Grace F, Yates M, Gomez R. Exercise, mood, self-efficacy, and social support as predictors of depressive symptoms in older adults: direct and interaction effects. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2145.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. King DW, Leskin GA, King LA, Weathers FW. Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD scale: evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):90.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Gold MS, Bentler PM. Treatments of missing data: a Monte Carlo comparison of RBHDI, iterative stochastic regression imputation, and expectation-maximization. Struct Equ Model. 2000;7(3):319–55.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the College of Education and the College of Arts and Social Sciences of the Mindanao State University – Iligan Institute of Technology (MSU-IIT) for their support in the conduct and writing of this manuscript.

Funding

The fund used in this research came from the authors’ budget.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IGM wrote the manuscript and headed the revisions. JGL conducted the data analysis and was a major contributor in writing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Imelu G. Mordeno.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Only participants who signed assent forms and obtained informed consent from their parents were allowed to participate in the study. The study was reviewed and approved by an ethics committee from the Mindanao State University – Iligan Institute of Technology College of Education, Philippines. The procedures used in this study adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mordeno, I.G., Luzano, J. Examining specific and non-specific symptoms of the best-fitting posttraumatic stress disorder model in conflict-exposed adolescents. BMC Psychol 11, 353 (2023). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-023-01389-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-023-01389-8

Keywords