Skip to main content
  • Study Protocol
  • Open access
  • Published:

Transdiagnostic phenomena of psychopathology in the context of the RDoC: protocol of a multimodal cross-sectional study

Abstract

In the past, affective and cognitive processes related to psychopathology have been examined within the boundaries of phenotype-based diagnostic labels, which has led to inconsistent findings regarding their underlying operating principles. Investigating these processes dimensionally in healthy individuals and by means of multiple modalities may provide additional insights into the psychological and neuronal mechanisms at their core. The transdiagnostic phenomena Neuroticism and Rumination are known to be closely linked. However, the exact nature of their relationship remains to be elucidated. The same applies to the associations between Hedonic Capacity, Negativity Bias and different Emotion Regulation strategies.

This multimodal cross-sectional study examines the relationship of the transdiagnostic phenomena Neuroticism and Rumination as well as Hedonic Capacity, the Negativity Bias and Emotion Regulation from a RDoC (Research Domain Criteria) perspective. A total of 120 currently healthy subjects (past 12 months) will complete several questionnaires regarding personality, emotion regulation, hedonic capacity, and psychopathologies as well as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during cognitive and emotional processing, to obtain data on the circuit, behavioral and self-report level.

This study aims to contribute to the understanding of the relationship between cognitive and affective processes associated with psychopathologies as well as their neuronal correlates. Ultimately, a grounded understanding of these processes could guide improvement of diagnostic labels and treatments. Limitations include the cross-sectional design and the limited variability in psychopathology scores due to the restriction of the sample to currently healthy subjects.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Mental disorders, especially internalizing disorders, are one of the leading causes of disability, affecting one in eight individuals worldwide, with numbers still rising [1,2,3,4,5] and therefore posing a significant public health issue. Currently, mental disorders are classified using phenomenologically based taxonomies such as the ICD or DSM [6, 7]. Although this approach has been useful in the past, patients falling under the same diagnostic label can show highly heterogenous symptoms and responses to the same treatment. Accordingly, new approaches attempting to tap into the neurobehavioral entities and mechanisms underlying mental disorders have been developed to provide more informative diagnostic labels and improve treatments.

The Research Domain Criteria initiative (RDoC [8, 9];), founded by the National Institute of Mental Health, provides a framework to investigate psychopathology as varying degrees of dysfunction in distinct psychological and biological systems. Namely the Positive Valence Systems, Negative Valence Systems, Cognitive Systems, Arousal and Regulatory Systems, Sensorimotor Systems and Social Processes constitute the general neurobehavioral domains of the RDoC. Each of the systems is in turn distinguished by so called (sub)constructs. The heterogenous and complex phenomenology of mental disorders is assumed to be driven by different degrees of aberrancy in these systems and their respective (sub)constructs. Investigating the operating principles of these systems dimensionally and across multiple modalities is at the core of RDoC, and accounts for the various sources of variability in findings regarding mental disorders and neurobehavioral functioning in general.

As the RDoC delineates a thorough framework to investigate neurobehavioral function in distinct systems through multiple units of analysis ranging from the level of genetics to self-reports, RDoC inspired research promises comprehensive insights into the underlying mechanisms associated with psychopathology. Applying the RDoC approach to the study of not only patients but also healthy volunteers (HV), may provide additional insight into these mechanisms. This RDoC inspired multimodal cross-sectional study of Positive Valence Systems, Negative Valence Systems and Cognitive Systems aims to elucidate the underlying mechanisms of transdiagnostic phenomena on the circuit, behavioral and self-report level in healthy subjects.

Negative valence systems: interaction of neuroticism and rumination

Neuroticism

The Negative Valence Systems (NVS; [8, 10]) domain of the RDoC matrix encompasses the neurobehavioral systems centrally involved in the response to (potential) punishment and aversive situations or contexts in general. Several heritable and transtemporally stable dispositional traits are known to correspond to that definition of the NVS. For instance, the concepts of Neuroticism / Negative Emotionality in the Big Five model of personality [11, 12], Eysenck’s Neuroticism [13], Negative Affect [14, 15] and the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) in the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST; [16,17,18,19]) all encompass the processing of aversive emotional material. However, the degree to which these constructs are related to neurobiologically informed entities varies substantially, with Neuroticism being primarily defined based on self-report data, whereas the BIS and NVS conceptualizations are primarily defined based on neuroscientific evidence. Although the definitions of these different constructs may vary in detail, they all include the processing of aversive information and can therefore being considered components of the same underlying latent factor [20,21,22]. Accordingly, in the following these different conceptualizations are summed up with the term Neuroticism. Neuroticism has been conceptualized as a subordinate component of the general factor of psychopathology, which is evident by associations with various mental disorders, with particular relevance to internalizing disorders like depression and anxiety [23, 24].

Originally, reactivity of the amygdala in response to negative emotional stimuli has been proposed to underly Neuroticism [13, 25,26,27,28,29], but meta-analytic results did not corroborate these findings [30]. More recent evidence points to a negative association of Neuroticism with increased connectivity between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (PFC), reflecting the impaired ability of the PFC to regulate amygdala reactivity in individuals showing higher expressions of Neuroticism [31,32,33,34,35,36]. An integrative approach by Williams [37] has suggested dysfunctions in an affective network comprising the anterior cingulate cortex, medial PFC, insula, and amygdalae to be associated with negative affect. Also, the neurobiologically informed RST of personality [16,17,18,19] has stimulated considerable research regarding the involvement of the hippocampi in the processing of aversive states, particularly anxiety. Although promising biomarkers of Neuroticism have been identified, our understanding of the underlying neurobehavioral operating principles is still relatively scarce.

Rumination

Repetitive negative thinking is characterized by a diminished ability to disengage from spontaneous thoughts and poses a transdiagnostic risk factor with profound implications for mental health, mostly studied in the forms of Rumination and Worry [38,39,40]. Rumination has been assigned to the NVS in the RDoC framework [10] and is conceptualized as the repetitive and passive dwelling on causes and consequences of distressing events. It has been shown to be a risk factor for internalizing disorders in general and depression more specifically [41,42,43,44,45,46]. Rumination can be probed either as a trait, reflecting the propensity to use ruminative thinking in response to distressing experiences, or as a state, displaying the current proficiency of ruminative processes. Although often employed by individuals to cope with negative events, Rumination elicits opposing effects by mitigation of problem solving and instrumental behavior resulting in enhanced negative thinking and mood [42]. Worry, however, is characterized by a repetitive thinking about possible negative events in the future and has been primarily associated with anxiety [38]. Substantial correlations between self-reported Worry and Rumination [47, 48] as well as shared neural circuits [49, 50] have raised doubts whether they are to be considered independent processes with distinct psychopathological trajectories.

The default mode network (DMN; [51, 52]) is a large-scale neural network becoming active when no cognitively demanding tasks are carried out and is most commonly identified using resting state functional magnet resonance imaging (fMRI). Due to its implication in mind wandering and daydreaming, the DMN is proposed to resemble self-oriented internally focused spontaneous thought [40, 53, 54]. The DMN encompasses prefrontal regions, the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, temporal regions, the hippocampi as well as parietal areas and angular gyri [37, 53,54,55]. Both, increased activity, and connectivity of the DMN have been reliably associated with Rumination and Worry during the resting-state as well as during Rumination induction tasks [49, 56,57,58,59,60,61]. Aberrant connectivities of the DMN, especially of the mPFC, have been consistently linked to depressive disorders [62, 63]. Moreover, the impaired ability to flexibly alternate between the DMN and task positive networks has been suggested to reflect the diminished ability to disengage from Rumination in depressed subjects [37, 60, 64,65,66,67,68,69].

Neuroticism-rumination-interaction

The interplay of Neuroticism and Rumination has primarily been investigated in the context of mediation models with Rumination mediating the relationship between Neuroticism and psychopathology [58, 70,71,72,73,74,75,76]. However, prior research has also pointed to Neuroticism and Rumination being related but distinct constructs, that exert effects on psychopathology independently from each other [63, 77]. Molecular approaches support this notion by showing that Neuroticism and Rumination are genetically related but separate over time in response to environmental influences [78, 79]. Although mediation models have been proven useful in elucidating the relationship of Neuroticism and Rumination, their distinctive and independent effects may be better represented in the context of moderation models. We therefore propose an amplification hypothesis: Neuroticism, here conceptualized as a distal higher-order vulnerability factor, exerts a direct effect on internalizing psychopathology. Rumination, here conceptualized as a dysfunctional coping mechanism and therefore more proximal risk factor, also exerts a direct effect on internalizing psychopathology independent from Neuroticism. If both coincide, their effects are hypothesized to potentiate and introduce additional risk for the respective person to develop internalizing symptoms, over and above Neuroticism and Rumination individually. Initial research suggesting direct as well as conditional effects of Neuroticism and Rumination fuels this hypothesis [80,81,82,83]. Additionally, a comparison of the moderation and mediation models to determine which model better explains the data could provide empirical evidence for the underlying nature of the association between Rumination and Neuroticism.

To our current knowledge, no neuroimaging studies have been conducted regarding the relationship of Neuroticism and Rumination. We therefore want to thoroughly elucidate their interrelatedness on the circuit as well as self-report level.

The negativity bias in the context of cognitive emotion regulation and anhedonia

The Cognitive Systems [8, 9] domain in the RDoC matrix encompasses various cognitive processes, such as attention, perception, and working memory. Working memory, the executive ability to store and process information, and emotions have a reciprocal and modulatory relationship. Thus, the effects of emotions on working memory capacity as well as the influence of working memory on emotional experience have been highlighted [84]. Also, the effectiveness of cognitive emotion regulation and working memory performance are interconnected. On the one hand, this is attributed to the ability to update emotional information; on the other hand, working memory performance modulates the regulation of negative affect [85, 86]. Accordingly, reduced working memory capacity is associated with impaired cognitive emotion regulation [87]. In contrast, there is a positive relationship between working memory capacity and the use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies [88, 89].

Trait Negativity Bias is a transdiagnostic phenomenon that is relevant in affective, anxiety, schizophrenic, personality, and eating disorders [90,91,92,93]. This phenomenon is used to describe that negative emotional stimuli are more salient and, compared to positive or neutral stimuli, are processed preferentially [94]. In addition, faster reaction times are observed at the behavioral level, and increased reactivity of associated brain areas at the neural level [95, 96]. Effects of Negativity Bias are evident in attention, memory, and the interpretation of external information, among others [92, 97]. From an evolutionary perspective, the Negativity Bias underlies a formerly survival adaptive function [94], although it can affect mental health in maladaptive ways [98]. For example, in depressive spectrum disorders, Negativity Bias is associated with pathogenesis, maintenance, and recurrence [99, 100]. In numerous mental disorders, the Negativity Bias manifests itself with impairments in working memory performance in that negative stimuli are responded to more quickly and with a lower error rate than stimuli with positive or neutral valence [101]. This might be facilitated by reduced top-down cognitive regulation and increased activity of subcortical areas of emotion processing [99]. In contrast, an opposite effect of cognitive-affective processing has been reported in mentally healthy individuals, as stimuli with positive valence have been found to have improved accuracy and faster reaction times [102]. Independent of mental illness, associations of Negativity Bias with adverse childhood experiences and the personality trait neuroticism/negative emotionality have been highlighted [103,104,105]. For example, childhood neglect and maltreatment influence cognitive information processing in adulthood [106], and at higher levels of neuroticism, negative information is perceived more saliently than positive or neutral information [107].

At the neural level, during experimental working memory tasks, there is both increased functional connectivity within the frontoparietal or Central Executive Network (CEN) and increased activity in associated areas [108,109,110]. Primarily, this network is anchored in the lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC). This again highlights the connection between working memory and cognitive emotion regulation, since, for example, functional connectivity between the lPFC and the amygdala is primarily relevant during positive reappraisal (Berboth & Morawetz, 2021), and the lPFC is thus involved in emotional self-regulatory processes. Based on meta-analytic evidence, Reid et al. [109] subdivide the CEN into an anterior and posterior part. Both are associated with components of cognitive control and working memory, respectively, with posterior areas more relevant to the maintenance of information and anterior areas more relevant to the further processing of that information [109]. Changes in functional connectivity within the CEN are related to the control of attention and emotion regulation. In this regard, hypo-connectivity within this network correlates with a deterioration of both [60]. In mental disorders, neuronal activity is reduced during maintenance, flexible updating, and interference control of information. This reduced activity is detected in areas directly associated with working memory, such as the lPFC and the anterior insula, as well as in the striatum [111]. Currently, there is no scientific evidence for the neural correlates of Negativity Bias in working memory, except that the lPFC is more activated when processing negative and positive information compared to neutral information [96]. However, a link between adverse childhood experiences and the Negativity Bias has already been highlighted, which is associated with increased activity in the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex [103].

The Positive Valence Systems [8, 9] are primarily responsible for responses to positive motivational situations or contexts, such as reward seeking, consummatory behavior, and reward/habit learning. Deviant reward processing is also a transdiagnostic phenomenon found in affective, schizophrenic, and post-traumatic stress disorders [112, 113]. Reward processes not only influence goal-directed attention [114] but also function as a possible top-down emotion regulation strategy [115]. Here, it is not only the initial hedonic response but more importantly the anticipation of reward [113]. For example, mental disorders are characterized by less salience of rewards and inadequate cognitive reactivation of rewarding past experiences [116]. At higher levels of Negativity Bias, rewarding stimuli are less salient and, moreover, are used in a reduced way for emotion regulation, which is associated with reduced reward anticipation [92, 117]. Hedonic capacity implies an individual's general response to appetitive stimuli and the motivation to achieve them. The negative manifestation of hedonic capacity is Anhedonia, i.e., the inability to feel pleasure or the diminished desire to engage in pleasurable activities [118]. Hedonic capacity can therefore always be evaluated in different contexts, such as the pleasure or lack of pleasure derived from sensory, social, and consumption-related events or the pursuit of hobbies. The transdiagnostic and typically treatment-resistant symptom of Anhedonia, has a central function in reward processing [119]. Anhedonia influences appraisal, anticipation, and motivation to reward, which is primarily associated with the ventral striatum at the neural level [120, 121]. The association between Anhedonia and poorer cognitive functions can be explained by reduced connectivity of reward-related neural areas [122].

Using a meta-analytic review of functional MRI studies, it was shown that significantly overlapping neural circuits are activated regardless of the type of rewarding stimulus, suggesting a general Reward Network (RN; [123]. Primarily, this network is anchored in the ventral tegmental area and ventral striatum. Reduced reactivity of the ventral striatum to rewarding stimuli is a transdiagnostic clinical biomarker [124,125,126]. Schwarz et al. [126] also highlighted that pathologically inhibited reward anticipation is associated with both reduced ventral striatum activity and reduced RN functional connectivity. This again clarifies the link between reward anticipation and Anhedonia, as ventral striatum activity acts as a neural biomarker in both [121, 123]. Moreover, increased ventral striatum reactivity during reward anticipation is associated with improved cognitive and social functioning, whereas decreased striatal activity is associated with increased affective lability [126]. Decreased reward reactivity is further related to stress-induced Anhedonia, adverse childhood experiences, and maladaptive coping strategies [127].

Methods

Hypotheses

Neuroticism

First, increased activation of the bilateral amygdalae and hippocampi as well as significant functional connectivity between the amygdalae and the PFC are hypothesized during negative emotional processing. These neural measures are hypothesized to be in turn associated with self-reported Neuroticism. Moreover, both self-reports and neural measures of Neuroticism are hypothesized to predict symptoms of internalizing psychopathology.

Rumination

Increased activation, and functional connectivity within nodes of the DMN are hypothesized to be observed during Rumination. Additionally, an association between these neural measures and self-reported Rumination is assumed as well as the prediction of symptoms of internalizing psychopathology by means of these measures.

Neuroticism-rumination-interaction

The interaction between Neuroticism and Rumination on the self-report and circuit level is hypothesized to predict symptoms of internalizing psychopathology. Moreover, the interaction terms of Neuroticism and Rumination on the self-report level are hypothesized to be correlated with the interaction terms of Neuroticism and Rumination on the circuit level.

Negativity bias

In working memory, negative and positive words are preferentially processed over neutral words. This is accompanied by an improvement in accuracy and reaction time and is also associated with increased activity and connectivity of the CEN and implicated areas, such as the lPFC.

Cognitive emotion regulation

The manifestation of Negativity Bias is associated with increased use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, such as Rumination, catastrophizing, and blaming oneself and others. Whereas positivity bias, i.e. preferential processing of positive stimuli, is associated with increased use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, such as positive refocusing, reappraisal and refocusing on planning. Adaptive emotion regulation strategies are further related to increased activity and connectivity of the CEN and implicated areas, such as the lPFC.

Hedonic capacity

Negativity bias is positively related to Anhedonia, whereas positivity bias is associated with enhanced responsiveness to appetitive stimuli. Hedonic capacity is related to activity and connectivity of the RN and implicated areas, such as the ventral striatum, during reward anticipation. Anhedonia is associated with decreased activity and connectivity in these areas. Furthermore, a higher expression of the Negativity Bias inhibits the activation of the ventral striatum during reward anticipation. Finally, adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies are negatively related to Anhedonia.

Participants

Based on a power analysis using G*Power [128], a sample size of N = 109 subjects is required to detect associations of moderate magnitude (r = 0.3), assuming an alpha threshold of 0.05 and a power of 0.90. Additionally, the sample size is increased by 10% to account for possible dropouts, resulting in a target sample of 120 complete data sets.

Only healthy young adults between 18 and 45 years will be enrolled in this study. Exclusion criteria include a diagnosed mental disorder, psychotherapy or psychopharmacological treatment in the past 12 months, pregnancy, or meeting any of the MRI exclusion criteria. Additionally, methylxanthines like caffeine have been shown to affect neuronal circuits involved in the processing of rewards [129], which is why participants who consume more than 8 standard caffeinated drinks per day are excluded from participation. Furthermore, subjects are instructed to refrain from alcohol and other psychoactive substances 48 h before the MRI assessment as well as methylxanthines at the day of the MRI assessments. Subjects are recruited using e-mail, personal approach, and advertising of the study in the online management system for psychological studies conducted at MSB Medical School Berlin and Free University Berlin. The study has been approved by the institutional review board of the MSB Medical School Berlin and all subjects provide written informed consent to participate. Psychology students at the MSB Medical School Berlin or Free University Berlin receive course credits for their participation in this study. Enrollment of participants started on 1st of November 2022 and is currently ongoing. It is anticipated that the last subject will finish participation on 1st of November 2023.

Procedure

All subjects undergo two phases in this study. First, an online link is provided leading to an internet-based questionnaire (www.soscisurvey.de) to determine the eligibility of potential participants. In that vein, written informed consent to participate in this study is provided by the subjects, followed by the Mini DIPS [130] to ensure subjects do not currently suffer from mental disorders or have been in treatment for a mental disorder in the past 12 months. Moreover, employing a check list provided by the MRI site, subjects fulfilling any of the MRI exclusion criteria are denied further participation in the study for safety reasons.

After successfully determining the eligibility of the subjects, a second online link is provided to complete questionnaire set 1 encompassing a variety of constructs related to psychopathology, transdiagnostic phenomena and other variables of interest. The respective section below gives a full description of the questionnaires in use. Questionnaire set 1 takes about 60 min to complete.

Third, subjects receive an appointment for the MRI measurements taking place at the Center for Cognitive Neuroscience Berlin. After arrival, participants first complete additional self-reports for about 60 min, followed by the MRI assessment.

During the MRI assessment, first, an anatomical scan is conducted followed by a resting-state sequence. The resting-state measurement is implemented as the first functional sequence to rule out carry-over effects elicited by emotionally valenced material from the tasks. Then, the SID task [131] and the EMOBACK task [96, 132] are used to investigate neural responses to social reward anticipation and cognition-emotion interactions, respectively. Subsequently, the Emotional Faces Task employing human faces expressing negative emotions, which has been shown to robustly activate the bilateral amygdala [96], is used to probe the processing of negative emotional stimuli in the brain. Lastly, the Rumination and Worry Induction Task (based on [58, 59, 133,134,135]) is employed to investigate brain activation and connectivity elicited by Rumination and Worry, respectively. Total duration of the MRI examination is 1.75 h and after completion, participants fill out questionnaire set 3 (duration 2 min). A visual depiction of the procedure can be found in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Procedure

SID, Social Incentive Delay Task; EMOBACK, Emotional N-back Task; EFT, Emotional Faces Task; RWIT, Rumination and Worry Induction Task

Questionnaires

Various questionnaires are employed to assess personality traits, coping styles, hedonic capacity, constructs related to psychopathology and covariates like socioeconomic status and need for cognition. An overview of the questionnaires is given in Table 1.

Table 1 Questionnaires

FMRI paradigms

Resting-state

During the 10 min resting-state, subjects are instructed to relax, to not think about anything in particular and let the mind wander, while looking at a fixation cross.

Social Incentive Delay Task (SID)

The SID task [131] investigates reward anticipation and consists of 88 blocks, that last approximately 10 min. Within each block, a possible reward is dependent on the subject's reaction time. This is evaluated in advance to allow for individual variability. At the beginning of a block, a cue stimulus is used to announce the possible reward, thus eliciting reward anticipation. This is done by presenting a circle with 1, 2, or 3 horizontal lines for 240 ms. The more lines the circle contains, the higher the potential reward. As a control condition, the presentation of a triangle serves as a cue that no gain can be obtained in this block, regardless of the reaction time. The cue stimulus is followed by a pause of 2250–2750 ms, in which a crosshair is presented. Subsequently, a filled square is presented as the target stimulus for 160–260 ms (adapted to the individual reaction time) and the participants are asked to press a key as quickly as possible. Depending on the reaction time (with a cut-off at 66%, i.e., the key press must occur within 111–172 ms), the rewarding stimulus (1650 ms) follows after an ISI of 300 ms. This represents, depending on the cue stimulus and reaction time, a face with an increasingly happy expression (3 intensity levels). Faces are randomly sampled from a pool of 66 photos (22 different subjects (11 female, 11 male) with happy facial expressions of three differing intensities). Photos from the NimStim set of Facial Expressions database are used for this purpose (available at http://www.macbrain.org; [178]). If the reaction time was below the individual cut-off value, or the cue stimulus of the block was a triangle and thus a "no win" stimulus, only an unrecognizable face is presented using morphing software (see [131]).

Emotional n-back Task (EMOBACK)

This is an approximately twelve-minute n-back task using emotional words from the Berlin Affective Word List (BAWL; [179]). Words are classified as positive, negative, or neutral according to BAWL norms, with 35 different words assigned to each valence. All words are nouns and comparable in terms of length (5–8 letters), figurativeness, and frequency (total frequency of occurrence per million words). During the task, stimuli are presented block by block via a screen. Participants are asked to respond by pressing a key when the currently presented word matches the three-to-one (3-back). The blocks each consist of 15 words of one of the three valences and are presented for 500 ms with a 1500 ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI). Between blocks, there is a pause of 10–14 s during which a crosshair is seen on the screen. A total of 75 words are presented per condition. The order of the blocks is randomized.

Emotional Faces Task (EFT)

To investigate functional brain changes during emotional challenge, subjects are presented faces expressing the negative emotions fear, sadness, and disgust from the Warsaw Set of Emotional Facial Expression Pictures [180]. First, a brief training of the task is completed inside the scanner in advance of the data acquisition. During the task, a total of 72 emotional faces (counterbalanced for sex) are shown for 3 s each and scrambled pictures showing random color patterns with either a blue or a green frame are employed as the control condition. Subjects are instructed to report the sex of the depicted person or the color of the frame using button presses, to ensure sufficient task engagement. A total of 12 blocks with faces and scrambled pictures are presented in randomized order, with each block lasting for 18 s. During the inter-trial interval, a white fixation cross on a dark background is shown. Total task duration is 13 min.

Rumination and Worry Induction Task (RWIT)

Rumination and Worry are induced experimentally using the RWIT. In contrast to most rumination induction tasks [58, 59, 134], the RWIT aims to not only elicit Rumination, but also Worry. Since recent studies have proposed a relationship between Rumination and negative mood in HV [58, 181]. Hence, participants complete a dysphoric mood induction procedure prior to the start of the RWIT, by recalling a situation, which elicited negative emotions like guilt, shame, sadness, or fear, and vividly remembering this situation for 2 min. Next, participants are instructed to think about the negative event by means of self-oriented questions proposed to elicit Rumination and Worry, respectively. For instance, an item proposed to elicit Rumination is “Why can't I handle events like this better?” and for Worry “How can the event affect my future negatively?”. As contrast condition, subjects are shown 4 brief sentences describing a situation and instructed to vividly imagine it, for example “A typical classroom from school”. Prior to every condition, a slide is shown on the screen for 30 s to explain the goal of the following condition. Each condition includes 4 questions and sentences, which are shown for 90 s. The Rumination and Worry condition are presented in a randomized order, whereas the control condition is always presented last to ensure that the experimental conditions are undergone continuously. In total, the RWIT takes 20 min.

MRI acquisition and analysis

MRI acquisition

Brain images are acquired using a 3 Tesla MRI scanner (PRISMA fit, Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) and a 32-channel coil. The anatomical image is acquired by means of a 3D T1 weighted sequence (Magnetization Prepared Rapid Acquisition Gradient Echo sequence, TR = 2.3 s, TE = 3.03 ms, slices = 192, voxel size = 1 × 1x1 mm, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 256 × 256x192 mm). Functional brain images are acquired using a T2* weighted gradient echo-planar imaging sequence sensitive to the BOLD effect (Blood Oxygen Level Dependent), accelerated by a factor of 3 (TR = 1 s, TE = 0.3 ms, slices = 39, voxel size = 3 × 3x3 mm, flip angle = 65°, matrix = 64 × 64, FOV = 192 × 192x140 mm, MB factor = 3). Moreover, to improve registration a fieldmap is obtained employing a double-echo gradient echo field map sequence (TR = 468 ms, TE = 4.92 / 7.38 ms, slices = 39, voxel size = 3 × 3x3 mm, flip angle = 60°, matrix = 64 × 64, FOV = 192 × 192x140).

MRI analysis

Brain image preprocessing and analysis are carried out using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool; [182,183,184]) version 6, as part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library; [185,186,187]).

Regions of Interest (ROIs)

The bilateral amygdalae, hippocampi as well as the PFC are defined as regions of interest for the main analyses of the EFT. During the RWIT, seeds within the DMN are defined as ROIs, specifically the posterior cingulate cortex as seed for the core-system and the hippocampi as seed for medial-temporal subsystem. ROIs related to the SID task for anticipation are the ventral striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, supplementary motor area, anterior insula, and ventral tegmental area. During the initial hedonic response to social reward, the dorsal caudate, supplementary motor areal, anterior insula, putamen, thalamus, and midbrain are relevant. For the EMOBACK task, the focus is on the lateral prefrontal cortex (up to BA45 & BA44), posterior medial prefrontal cortex, intraparietal cortex, anterior insula, and inferior frontal gyrus. For the connectivity analyses, both SID and EMOBACK ROIs are extracted from the ANIMA database [109]. For SID, the Reward Network [123], and for EMOBACK, the Central Executive Network [188].

Preprocessing

T1 anatomical data are biasfield corrected and aligned to the MNI152 standard space using linear alignment via FSL FLIRT with 12 degrees of freedom and subsequently refined non-linearly as implemented in FSL FNIRT. The processing of the functional brain images includes correction for participant head motion, correction for EPI distortions using fieldmap data, and a 5 mm FWHM spatial smoothing. To identify and correct for more subtle effects of head motion and noise induced by the multiband acceleration or physiological processes, ICA-based methods are used. Finally, a temporal 0.01 Hz high-pass filter is applied to remove scanner drifts. Furthermore, a transformation from the functional space to the T1 anatomical space using FSL Boundary Based Registration is obtained. Eventually, the transformation is combined with the T1 to MNI152 registration to transfer the functional data from the individual’s native space to the MNI152 standard space.

First-level

A General Linear Model implemented in FEAT is used on the preprocessed participant-level data in combination with Python [189] to obtain the contrasts of interest. First, the onsets of the respective task conditions are calculated from the log files and subsequently used by FEAT to model the task-specific BOLD signal changes. Thereafter, an estimate of the fit between the task regressor and the BOLD time series is obtained for each voxel, ultimately resulting in a map of statistically significant activations. Multiple comparisons correction is performed at the cluster level using Gaussian Random Field Theory (voxel: z > 3.1 uncorrected; cluster: p < 0.05, corrected). The resulting activation maps are used in subsequent group-level analyses. Moreover, to investigate functional connectivity between regions of interest and the rest of the brain during the tasks, a psychophysiological interaction analysis is conducted. The respective regressor is acquired by forming an interaction term of the time series of the voxel with the best fit to the task regressor in a given ROI as the seed and the task regressor of interest. Applying this new interaction regressor using the General Linear Model described above results in a connectivity map of regions significantly correlated with the seed region.

Group-level

A General Linear Model using FSL will be implemented to tests for statistically significant activations and connectivities. Multiple comparisons correction is performed at the cluster level (voxel: z > 3.1 uncorrected; cluster: p < 0.05, corrected). Moreover, ROI analyses are conducted using FSL featquery to extract BOLD percent signal changes and contrast of parameter estimates, to be used in later stages of the analysis pipeline.

Resting-state

For the analysis of resting-state functional connectivity the CONN toolbox [190] will be used to obtain seed-based correlation maps on the whole-brain level as well as connectivity measures between regions of interest to be used in later stages of the group-level analyses.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses going beyond the image processing will be conducted in R [191, 192] or Python libraries such as Pandas [193], Matplotlib [194], and SciPy [195]. Multiple comparisons correction is performed using the FDR method assuming α = 0.05.

Neuroticism-rumination-interaction

First, to represent the higher order nature of Neuroticism, structural equation modeling is used to generate a latent variable using the respective scales of the ATQ, BFI, PANAS trait as well as the ARES, SPSRQ, BIS/BAS, and RST-PQ. Subsequently, the results of the SEM are used on the individual level to remove variance from the data not specific to the latent variable, resulting in a denoised measure of Neuroticism.

Next, hypotheses are tested statistically using regression models adjusting for age and sex. The association of self-reported Neuroticism with amygdala and hippocampus reactivity as well as the functional connectivity of the amygdalae to the PFC during the EFT are tested using multiple regression. In that vein, the effect of Neuroticism as a generic risk factor for internalizing disorders is tested using linear models, including self-reported and neural measures of Neuroticism as predictors of internalizing symptomology as the dependent variables. A measure of internalizing symptomology will be obtained by calculating a standardized composite of the depression and anxiety scales of the DASS.

Similarly, the association between self-reported Rumination and DMN connectivity during the RWIT is tested by employing linear models. Additionally, the effect of Rumination as a transdiagnostic risk factor is evaluated using multiple regression, including self-report and neural measures of Rumination as predictors and internalizing psychopathology as the dependent variable.

Ultimately, a moderator analysis is used in the context of a multiple regression to investigate the interaction effect of Neuroticism and Rumination on internalizing psychopathology. Herein, the interaction term is formulated for the self-report and neural measures, respectively. Furthermore, an association between the interaction terms of the self-report and neural level is analyzed.

Exploratorily, the effect of Worry as a transdiagnostic risk factor is investigated. As already described in the introduction section, Worry is a cognitive process closely related to Rumination. Therefore, the analyses described above are repeated using Worry instead of Rumination to elucidate the interaction of Neuroticism and Worry on internalizing psychopathology. Additionally, DMN connectivity estimates derived from the resting-state instead of the RWIT are employed as neural measures of Rumination and used in the respective subsequent analyses. Finally, a model comparison approach is planned to determine whether the data is explained better by a moderation or a mediation model regarding the relationship between Neuroticism, Rumination, and psychopathology.

Negativity bias

To calculate a value for the negativity or positivity bias, the behavioral data of the EMOBACK task are utilized. The average accuracy and reaction time is used by calculating the difference between the negative and positive condition. These values will be z-standardized. A positive value implies the presence of a Negativity Bias, and a negative value implies a positivity bias. In the next step, the standardized values for accuracy and reaction time are examined for correlation with the functional neural data of the CEN during EMOBACK.

Cognitive emotion regulation

Neural data from the activity and connectivity analyses of the CEN especially the lPFC during the working memory task will be correlated with the two main scales of the CERQ (adaptive & maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies). In the following step, multiple regressions are performed, with the subscales of the adaptive and maladaptive strategies as predictors. Similarly, the association between Negativity Bias and cognitive emotion regulation strategies will be tested.

Hedonic capacity

Neural data from the activity and connectivity analyses of the RN especially the ventral striatum during the social reward anticipation task will be first correlated with hedonic capacity/tone main scales from LAPS and DARS questionnaires. In addition, linear regressions will be used to examine the relationship of activity/connectivity with the subscales of the LAPS (cognitive functioning, overall functioning, meaningful life, and happiness) and those of the DARS (social activities, sensory experiences, hobbies, and food/drink). Similarly, the association between Negativity Bias and hedonic capacity variables will be tested.

Discussion

Internalizing disorders are the most common type of mental disorders with substantial impact on public health [1, 5]. Many of these disorders are characterized by alterations in both cognitive and emotional processes [196]. In the past, cognitive and emotional processes associated with psychopathologies were investigated within the boundaries of phenomenologically based diagnostic criteria. However, recent developments highlight the need for a dimensional approach, rooted in multimodal assessment of neurobiologically informed entities of neurobehavioral functioning [8, 9]. Transdiagnostic phenomena such as Neuroticism, Rumination, Anhedonia, the Negativity Bias and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies are known to interact with each other, however current findings are contradictory and the exact nature of their relationships remain to be unknown [197]. This study aims to collect data on the circuit, behavioral and self-report level to investigate the operating principles of the neurobehavioral processes associated with psychopathology mentioned above.

A better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the relationship between Neuroticism and Rumination as well as hedonic capacity, Negativity Bias and emotion regulation strategies may guide future development of diagnostic labels and treatments.

Limitations of the present study include the cross-sectional design, which renders inferences to be correlational, so no causal conclusions regarding the relationship between the respective constructs of interest can be drawn. Since the sample is restricted to currently healthy subjects, no conclusions can be drawn regarding diagnosable mental disorders and limits the interindividual variability of the psychopathological phenomena in question. However, the requirements regarding the mental health of the sample are considerably low. Subjects are only required to not fulfill the criteria for diagnosis of a mental disorder or receive professional mental health interventions in the past 12 months to take part in this study. Since, diagnosis and treatment for mental disorders that lie further in the past are not taken into account and subsyndromal symptoms of mental distress are not sufficient to exclude subjects from participation. Therefore, measures of psychopathology are expected to show variability, at least to some extent.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

References

  1. Ferrari AJ, Charlson FJ, Norman RE, Patten SB, Freedman G, Murray CJL, et al. Burden of Depressive Disorders by Country, Sex, Age, and Year: Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Hay PJ, editor. PLoS Med. 2013;10(11):e1001547.

  2. James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, Abay SM, Abbafati C, Abbasi N, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. The Lancet. 2018;392(10159):1789–858.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Kessler RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Chatterji S, Lee S, Ormel J, et al. The global burden of mental disorders: An update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2011;18(1):23–33.

    Google Scholar 

  4. World Health Organisation. Fact sheets: Mental disorders. 2022. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-disorders.

  5. Vos T, Lim SS, Abbafati C, Abbas KM, Abbasi M, Abbasifard M, et al. Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet. 2020;396(10258):1204–22.

    Google Scholar 

  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth Edition. American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

  7. World Health Organisation. International Classification of Diseases (ICD). 2022. Available from: https://www.who.int/standards/classifications/classification-of-diseases.

  8. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):748–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. National Institute of Mental Health. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) 2021. Available from: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc.

  10. National Institute of Mental Health. Domain: Negative Valence Systems 2021. Available from: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc/constructs/negative-valence-systems.

  11. DeYoung CG. cybernetic big five theory. J Res Personal. 2015;56:33–58.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Rammstedt B, John OP. Kurzversion des Big Five Inventory (BFI-K). Diagnostica. 2005;51(4):195–206.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Eysenck H. The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and personality: origins and outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(1):122–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gray JA. The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behav Res Ther. 1970;8(3):249–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  18. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28(3):285–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. The neuropsychology of fear and anxiety: a foundation for Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. In: Corr PJ, editor. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality 1st ed. Cambridge University Press; 2008 p. 44–94. Available from: https://0-www-cambridge-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/core/product/identifier/CBO9780511819384A011/type/book_part. [cited 2023 May 23].

  20. Allen TA, DeYoung CG. Personality Neuroscience and the Five Factor Model. In: Widiger TA, editor. The Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model Oxford University Press; 2016 p. 319–50. Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/edited-volume/27987/chapter/211686820. [cited 2023 May 9].

  21. Gore WL, Widiger TA. Negative emotionality across diagnostic models: RDoC, DSM-5 Section III, and FFM. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat. 2018;9(2):155–64.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Watson D, Stanton K, Clark LA. Self-report indicators of negative valence constructs within the research domain criteria (RDoC): a critical review. J Affect Disord. 2017;216:58–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR, Bagby RM, et al. The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126(4):454–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, et al. Neuroticism and common mental disorders: Meaning and utility of a complex relationship. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(5):686–97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Canli T. Toward a neurogenetic theory of neuroticism. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129(1):153–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Cunningham WA, Arbuckle NL, Jahn A, Mowrer SM, Abduljalil AM. Aspects of neuroticism and the amygdala: chronic tuning from motivational styles. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(12):3399–404.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Everaerd D, Klumpers F, Van Wingen G, Tendolkar I, Fernández G. Association between neuroticism and amygdala responsivity emerges under stressful conditions. Neuroimage. 2015;112:218–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Miyakawa A, Knight RT, D’Esposito M. The influence of personality on neural mechanisms of observational fear and reward learning. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(11):2709–24.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(2):318–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Servaas MN, van der Velde J, Costafreda SG, Horton P, Ormel J, Riese H, et al. Neuroticism and the brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion processing. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1518–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cremers HR, Demenescu LR, Aleman A, Renken R, van Tol MJ, van der Wee NJA, et al. Neuroticism modulates amygdala—prefrontal connectivity in response to negative emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):963–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Deng Y, Li S, Zhou R, Walter M. Neuroticism modulates the functional connectivity from amygdala to frontal networks in females when avoiding emotional negative pictures. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;9(13):102.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, Gruber O. Fear is only as deep as the mind allows. Neuroimage. 2011;58(1):275–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Haas BW, Constable RT, Canli T. Stop the sadness: neuroticism is associated with sustained medial prefrontal cortex response to emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage. 2008;42(1):385–92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Silverman MH. Operationalizing neuroticism using task-based fmri and psychological measures in a large sample [Dissertation]. [Minneapolis, MN]: University of Minnesota; 2018.

  36. Yang J, Yu Y, Wang W, Qiu J. Atypical neural activation associated with implicit negative emotional facial processing in fMRI tasks in individuals with neuroticism personality traits. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(2):906–13.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Williams LM. Precision psychiatry: a neural circuit taxonomy for depression and anxiety. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(5):472–80.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Ehring T, Watkins ER. Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. Int J Cogn Ther. 2008;1(3):192–205.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Harvey AG, editor. Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders: a transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. 365 p.

  40. Marchetti I, Koster EHW, Klinger E, Alloy LB. Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: the dark side of the wandering mind. Clin Psychol Sci. 2016;4(5):835–57.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100(4):569–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008;3(5):400–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61(1):115–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Watkins ER. A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(6):589–609.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Rickerby N, Krug I, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, Forte E, Davenport R, Chayadi E, et al. Rumination across depression, anxiety, and eating disorders in adults: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2022.

  46. Sarin S, Abela J, Auerbach R. The response styles theory of depression: a test of specificity and causal mediation. Cogn Emot. 2005;19(5):751–61.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Hur J, Heller W, Kern JL, Berenbaum H. A Bi-factor approach to modeling the structure of worry and rumination. J Exp Psychopathol. 2017;8(3):252–64.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Spinhoven P, Drost J, Van Hemert B, Penninx BW. Common rather than unique aspects of repetitive negative thinking are related to depressive and anxiety disorders and symptoms. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;33:45–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Feurer C, Jimmy J, Chang F, Langenecker SA, Phan KL, Ajilore O, et al. Resting state functional connectivity correlates of rumination and worry in internalizing psychopathologies. Depress Anxiety. 2021;38(5):488–97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Makovac E, Fagioli S, Rae CL, Critchley HD, Ottaviani C. Can’t get it off my brain: meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on perseverative cognition. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2020;295: 111020.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003;100(1):253–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2001;98(2):676–82.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(3):251–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65(4):550–62.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Mak LE, Minuzzi L, MacQueen G, Hall G, Kennedy SH, Milev R. The default mode network in healthy individuals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Connect. 2017;7(1):25–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Berman MG, Misic B, Buschkuehl M, Kross E, Deldin PJ, Peltier S, et al. Does resting-state connectivity reflect depressive rumination? A tale of two analyses NeuroImage. 2014;103:267–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Burkhouse KL, Jacobs RH, Peters AT, Ajilore O, Watkins ER, Langenecker SA. Neural correlates of rumination in adolescents with remitted major depressive disorder and healthy controls. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2017;17(2):394–405.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Chen MH, Chang WC, Lin WC, Tu PC, Li CT, Bai YM, et al. Functional dysconnectivity of frontal cortex to striatum predicts ketamine infusion response in treatment-resistant depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;23(12):791–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Cooney RE, Joormann J, Eugène F, Dennis EL, Gotlib IH. Neural correlates of rumination in depression. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10(4):470–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Kaiser RH, Andrews-Hanna JR, Wager TD, Pizzagalli DA. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiat. 2015;72(6):603.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Zhou HX, Chen X, Shen YQ, Li L, Chen NX, Zhu ZC, et al. Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression. Neuroimage. 2020;206: 116287.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Doucet GE, Janiri D, Howard R, O’Brien M, Andrews-Hanna JR, Frangou S. Transdiagnostic and disease-specific abnormalities in the default-mode network hubs in psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional imaging studies. Eur Psychiatry. 2020;63(1): e57.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Wang D, Liu T, Shi J. Neural dynamic responses of monetary and social reward processes in adolescents. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020;21(14):141.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Belleau EL, Taubitz LE, Larson CL. Imbalance of default mode and regulatory networks during externally focused processing in depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(5):744–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Grimm S, Boesiger P, Beck J, Schuepbach D, Bermpohl F, Walter M, et al. Altered negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network during emotion processing in depressed subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(4):932–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Hamilton JP, Furman DJ, Chang C, Thomason ME, Dennis E, Gotlib IH. Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(4):327–33.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Koster EHW, De Lissnyder E, De Raedt R. Rumination is characterized by valence-specific impairments in switching of attention. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2013;144(3):563–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Liu C, Belleau EL, Dong D, Sun X, Xiong G, Pizzagalli DA, et al. Trait- and state-like co-activation pattern dynamics in current and remitted major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2023;337:159–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Philippi CL, Leutzinger K, Pessin S, Cassani A, Mikel O, Walsh EC, et al. Neural signal variability relates to maladaptive rumination in depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2022;156:570–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Iqbal N, Dar KA. Negative affectivity, depression, and anxiety: does rumination mediate the links? J Affect Disord. 2015;181:18–23.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Li Y, Xu Y, Chen Z. Effects of the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), behavioral activation system (BAS), and emotion regulation on depression: a one-year follow-up study in Chinese adolescents. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230(2):287–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Merino H, Senra C, Ferreiro F. Are worry and rumination specific pathways linking neuroticism and symptoms of anxiety and depression in patients with generalized anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder and mixed anxiety-depressive disorder? Soriano-Mas C, editor. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(5): e0156169.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Mohammadkhani S, Attar F, Akbari M. The linkage between negative affectivity with emotional distress in college student: The mediator and moderator role of difficulty in emotion regulation, repetitive negative thinking, and emotional invalidation. Curr Psychol. 2021;8(42):6216–28.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Ranđelović K, Ćirović N. Social anxiety and rumination in the context of the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory and the mediation model of social anxiety. Psihologija. 2022;55(1):1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Tortella-Feliu M, Balle M, Sesé A. Relationships between negative affectivity, emotion regulation, anxiety, and depressive symptoms in adolescents as examined through structural equation modeling. J Anxiety Disord. 2010;24(7):686–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Verstraeten K, Vasey MW, Raes F, Bijttebier P. Temperament and risk for depressive symptoms in adolescence: mediation by rumination and moderation by effortful control. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(3):349–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Whisman MA, du Pont A, Butterworth P. Longitudinal associations between rumination and depressive symptoms in a probability sample of adults. J Affect Disord. 2020;260:680–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Du Pont A, Rhee SH, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, Friedman NP. Are rumination and neuroticism genetically or environmentally distinct risk factors for psychopathology? J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(5):385–96.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Forthman KL, Kuplicki R, Yeh HW, Khalsa SS, Paulus MP, Guinjoan SM. Transdiagnostic behavioral and genetic contributors to repetitive negative thinking: a machine learning approach. J Psychiatr Res. 2023;162:207–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Feldner MT, Leen-Feldner EW, Zvolensky MJ, Lejuez CW. Examining the association between rumination, negative affectivity, and negative affect induced by a paced auditory serial addition task. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2006;37(3):171–87.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Merino H, Ferreiro F, Senra C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional symptoms: reconsidering the role of worry and rumination. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36(1):136–42.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Vidal-Arenas V, Bravo AJ, Ortet-Walker J, Ortet G, Mezquita L, Ibáñez MI, et al. Neuroticism, rumination, depression and suicidal ideation: a moderated serial mediation model across four countries. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2022;22(3): 100325.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Zvolensky MJ, Paulus DJ, Bakhshaie J, Garza M, Ochoa-Perez M, Lemaire C, et al. Interactive effect of negative affectivity and rumination in terms of mental health among latinos in primary care. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2016;3(4):646–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Mikels JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Affective working memory: an integrative psychological construct. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2019;14(4):543–59.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Hendricks MA, Buchanan TW. Individual differences in cognitive control processes and their relationship to emotion regulation. Cogn Emot. 2016;30(5):912–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Pe ML, Raes F, Kuppens P. The cognitive building blocks of emotion regulation: ability to update working memory moderates the efficacy of rumination and reappraisal on emotion. Di Pellegrino G, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(7):e69071.

  87. Malagoli C, Usai MC. The effects of gender and age on inhibition and working memory organization in 14- to 19-year-old adolescents and young adults. Cogn Dev. 2018;45:10–23.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Peckham AD, Johnson SL. Cognitive control training for emotion-related impulsivity. Behav Res Ther. 2018;105:17–26.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Schweizer S, Samimi Z, Hasani J, Moradi A, Mirdoraghi F, Khaleghi M. Improving cognitive control in adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Behav Res Ther. 2017;93:88–94.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Nikendei C, Weisbrod M, Schild S, Bender S, Walther S, Herzog W, et al. Anorexia nervosa: Selective processing of food-related word and pictorial stimuli in recognition and free recall tests. Int J Eat Disord. 2008;41(5):439–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Peters MJV, Hauschildt M, Moritz S, Jelinek L. Impact of emotionality on memory and meta-memory in schizophrenia using video sequences. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2013;44(1):77–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Williams LM, Gatt JM, Schofield PR, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E. ‘Negativity bias’ in risk for depression and anxiety: brain–body fear circuitry correlates, 5-HTT-LPR and early life stress. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):804–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Winter D, Elzinga B, Schmahl C. Emotions and memory in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology. 2014;47(2):71–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Vaish A, Grossmann T, Woodward A. Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(3):383–403.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Carretié L, Mercado F, Tapia M, Hinojosa JA. Emotion, attention, and the ‘negativity bias’, studied through event-related potentials. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001;41(1):75–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Hartling C, Metz S, Pehrs C, Scheidegger M, Gruzman R, Keicher C, et al. Comparison of Four fMRI paradigms probing emotion processing. Brain Sci. 2021;11(5):525.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Everaert J, Podina IR, Koster EHW. A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;58:33–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Beck AT, Bredemeier K. A unified model of depression: integrating clinical, cognitive, biological, and evolutionary perspectives. Clin Psychol Sci. 2016;4(4):596–619.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Disner SG, Beevers CG, Haigh EAP, Beck AT. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(8):467–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. LeMoult J, Gotlib IH. Depression: a cognitive perspective. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;69:51–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Levens SM, Gotlib IH. Updating positive and negative stimuli in working memory in depression. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2010;139(4):654–64.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Lindström BR, Bohlin G. Emotion processing facilitates working memory performance. Cogn Emot. 2011;25(7):1196–204.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Metz S, Aust S, Fan Y, Bönke L, Harki Z, Gärtner M, et al. The influence of early life stress on the integration of emotion and working memory. Behav Brain Res. 2018;339:179–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Norris CJ, Leaf PT, Fenn KM. Negativity bias in false memory: moderation by neuroticism after a delay. Cogn Emot. 2019;33(4):737–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Vrijsen JN, Van Amen CT, Koekkoek B, Van Oostrom I, Schene AH, Tendolkar I. Childhood trauma and negative memory bias as shared risk factors for psychopathology and comorbidity in a naturalistic psychiatric patient sample. Brain Behav. 2017;7(6): e00693.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Gibb BE, Schofield CA, Coles ME. Reported history of childhood abuse and young adults’ information-processing biases for facial displays of emotion. Child Maltreat. 2009;14(2):148–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Sutherland MR, Mather M. Negative arousal increases the effects of stimulus salience in older adults. Exp Aging Res. 2015;41(3):259–71.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  108. Habas C, Kamdar N, Nguyen D, Prater K, Beckmann CF, Menon V, et al. Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J Neurosci. 2009;29(26):8586–94.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Reid AT, Bzdok D, Genon S, Langner R, Müller VI, Eickhoff CR, et al. ANIMA: a data-sharing initiative for neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neuroimage. 2016;124:1245–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable Intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349–56.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Yaple ZA, Tolomeo S, Yu R. Mapping working memory-specific dysfunction using a transdiagnostic approach. NeuroImage Clin. 2021;31: 102747.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Sailer U, Robinson S, Fischmeister FPhS, König D, Oppenauer C, Lueger-Schuster B, et al. Altered reward processing in the nucleus accumbens and mesial prefrontal cortex of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(11):2836–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Whitton AE, Treadway MT, Pizzagalli DA. Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2015;28(1):7–12.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  114. Yao Y, Xuan Y, Wu R, Sang B. Regulatory effects of reward anticipation and target on attention processing of emotional stimulation. Front Psychol. 2020;3(11):1170.

    Google Scholar 

  115. Kelley NJ, Glazer JE, Pornpattananangkul N, Nusslock R. Reappraisal and suppression emotion-regulation tendencies differentially predict reward-responsivity and psychological well-being. Biol Psychol. 2019;140:35–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Pizzagalli DA. Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10(1):393–423.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  117. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6(1):285–312.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Pizzagalli DA. Toward a better understanding of the mechanisms and pathophysiology of anhedonia: are we ready for translation? Am J Psychiatry. 2022;179(7):458–69.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Kelly CA, Freeman KB, Schumacher JA. Treatment-resistant depression with anhedonia: integrating clinical and preclinical approaches to investigate distinct phenotypes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;136: 104578.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Der-Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35(1):68–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Gorwood P. Neurobiological mechanisms of anhedonia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2022;10(3):291–9.

    Google Scholar 

  122. Felger JC, Li Z, Haroon E, Woolwine BJ, Jung MY, Hu X, et al. Inflammation is associated with decreased functional connectivity within corticostriatal reward circuitry in depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(10):1358–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Gu R, Huang W, Camilleri J, Xu P, Wei P, Eickhoff SB, et al. Love is analogous to money in human brain: coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses of social and monetary reward anticipation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:108–28.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Baskin-Sommers AR, Foti D. Abnormal reward functioning across substance use disorders and major depressive disorder: considering reward as a transdiagnostic mechanism. Int J Psychophysiol. 2015;98(2):227–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Grimm O, Van Rooij D, Hoogman M, Klein M, Buitelaar J, Franke B, et al. Transdiagnostic neuroimaging of reward system phenotypes in ADHD and comorbid disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;128:165–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Schwarz K, Moessnang C, Schweiger JI, Baumeister S, Plichta MM, Brandeis D, et al. Transdiagnostic prediction of affective, cognitive, and social function through brain reward anticipation in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression, and autism spectrum diagnoses. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(3):592–602.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Corral-Frías NS, Nikolova YS, Michalski LJ, Baranger DAA, Hariri AR, Bogdan R. Stress-related anhedonia is associated with ventral striatum reactivity to reward and transdiagnostic psychiatric symptomatology. Psychol Med. 2015;45(12):2605–17.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Volkow ND, Morales M. The brain on drugs: from reward to addiction. Cell. 2015;162(4):712–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  130. Margraf J. Mini-DIPS: Diagnostisches Kurz-Interview bei psychischen Störungen. 1st ed. Berlin: Springer; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Spreckelmeyer KN, Krach S, Kohls G, Rademacher L, Irmak A, Konrad K, et al. Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(2):158–65.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  132. Grimm S, Weigand A, Kazzer P, Jacobs AM, Bajbouj M. Neural mechanisms underlying the integration of emotion and working memory. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):1188–94.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Kowalski J, Wypych M, Marchewka A, Dragan M. Neural correlates of cognitive-attentional syndrome: an fMRI study on repetitive negative thinking induction and resting state functional connectivity. Front Psychol. 2019;26(10):648.

    Google Scholar 

  134. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cogn Emot. 1993;7(6):561–70.

    Google Scholar 

  135. Paulesu E, Sambugaro E, Torti T, Danelli L, Ferri F, Scialfa G, et al. Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: a functional MRI study. Psychol Med. 2010;40(1):117–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Wiltink J, Vogelsang U, Beutel ME. Temperament and personality: the German version of the Adult Temperament Questionaire (ATQ). GMS Psycho-Soc-Med. 2006;3:Doc10.

    Google Scholar 

  137. Hartig J, Moosbrugger H. Die, “ARES-Skalen” zur Erfassung der individuellen BIS- und BAS-Sensitivität. Z Für Differ Diagn Psychol. 2003;24(4):293–310.

    Google Scholar 

  138. Danner D, Rammstedt B, Bluemke M, Treiber L, Berres S, Soto C, et al. Die deutsche Version des Big Five Inventory 2 (BFI-2). Zusammenstellung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Items Skalen ZIS. 2016.

  139. Danner D, Rammstedt B, Bluemke M, Lechner C, Berres S, Knopf T, et al. Das big five inventar 2: validierung eines Persönlichkeitsinventars zur Erfassung von 5 Persönlichkeitsdomänen und 15 Facetten. Diagnostica. 2019;65(3):121–32.

    Google Scholar 

  140. Soto CJ, John OP. The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017;113(1):117–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  141. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral Inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(2):319–33.

    Google Scholar 

  142. Strobel A, Beauducel A, Debener S, Brocke B. Eine deutschsprachige Version des BIS/BAS-Fragebogens von Carver und White. Z Für Differ Diagn Psychol. 2001;22(3):216–27.

    Google Scholar 

  143. Hewig J, Hagemann D. Der SPSRQ-Fragebogen von Torrubia, Ávila. Moltó und Caseras - unveröffentlichte deutsche Übersetzung: Universität Trier; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  144. Torrubia R, Ávila C, Moltó J, Caseras X. The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personal Individ Differ. 2001;31(6):837–62.

    Google Scholar 

  145. Breyer B, Bluemke M. Deutsche Version der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule PANAS (GESIS Panel). Zusammenstellung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Items Skalen ZIS 2016. Available from: http://zis.gesis.org/DoiId/zis242. [cited 2023 Apr 6].

  146. Corr PJ, Cooper AJ. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality Questionnaire (RST-PQ): development and validation. Psychol Assess. 2016;28(11):1427–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  147. Pugnaghi G, Cooper A, Ettinger U, Corr PJ. The psychometric properties of the German language Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory-Personality Questionnaire (RST-PQ). J Individ Differ. 2018;39(3):182–90.

    Google Scholar 

  148. Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Schönfeld S, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(2):225–32.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  149. Bürger C, Kühner C. Copingstile im Umgang mit depressiver Stimmung. Z Für Klin Psychol Psychother. 2007;36(1):36–45.

    Google Scholar 

  150. Kühner C, Huffziger S, Nolen-Hoeksema S. RSQ-D: Response Styles Questionaire -Deutsche Version-. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  151. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cogn Ther Res. 2003;27:247–59.

    Google Scholar 

  152. Marchetti I, Mor N, Chiorri C, Koster EHW. The Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI): validation and psychometric evaluation. Cogn Ther Res. 2018;42(4):447–60.

    Google Scholar 

  153. Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD. Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behav Res Ther. 1990;28(6):487–95.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Stöber J. Besorgnis: Ein Vergleich dreier Inventare zur Erfassung allgemeiner Sorgen. Z Für Differ Diagn Psychol. 1995;16(1):50–63.

    Google Scholar 

  155. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personal Individ Differ. 2001;30(8):1311–27.

    Google Scholar 

  156. Loch N, Hiller W, Witthöft M. Der Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ): Erste teststatistische Überprüfung einer deutschen Adaption. Z Für Klin Psychol Psychother. 2011;40(2):94–106.

    Google Scholar 

  157. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13(1):27–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  158. Michalak J, Zarbock G, Drews M, Otto D, Mertens D, Ströhle G, et al. Erfassung von Achtsamkeit mit der deutschen Version des Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaires (FFMQ-D). Z Für Gesundheitspsychologie. 2016;24(1):1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  159. Franz M, Lemke M, Meyer T, Ulferts J, Puhl P, Snaith R. Deutsche Version der Snaith-Hamilton-Pleasure-Scale (SHAPS-D). Fortschritte Neurol Psychiatr. 1998;66(09):407–13.

    Google Scholar 

  160. Snaith RP, Hamilton M, Morley S, Humayan A, Hargreaves D, Trigwell P. A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the snaith-hamilton pleasure scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167(1):99–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Demyttenaere K, Mortier P, Kiekens G, Bruffaerts R. Is there enough “interest in and pleasure in” the concept of depression? The development of the Leuven Affect and Pleasure Scale (LAPS). CNS Spectr. 2019;24(02):265–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  162. Rizvi SJ, Quilty LC, Sproule BA, Cyriac A, Michael Bagby R, Kennedy SH. Development and validation of the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS) in a community sample and individuals with major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2015;229(1–2):109–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  163. Wellan SA, Daniels A, Walter H. State anhedonia in young healthy adults: psychometric properties of the German Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS) and effects of the COVID-19 Pandemic. Front Psychol. 2021;23(12): 682824.

    Google Scholar 

  164. Margraf J, Cwik JC, Pflug V, Schneider S. Structured clinical interviews for mental disorders across the lifespan: Psychometric quality and further developments of the DIPS Open Access interviews.[Strukturierte klinische Interviews zur Erfassung psychischer Störungen über die Lebensspanne: Gütekriterien und Weiterentwicklungen der DIPS-Verfahren]. Z Für Klin Psychol Psychother. 2017;46(3):176–86.

    Google Scholar 

  165. Antony MM, Bieling PJ. Psychometric properties of the 42-Item and 21-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):176–81.

    Google Scholar 

  166. Crawford JR, Henry JD. The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS): normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2003;42(2):111–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  167. Nilges P, Essau C. Die Depressions-Angst-Stress-Skalen: Der DASS – ein Screeningverfahren nicht nur für Schmerzpatienten. Schmerz. 2015;29(6):649–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  168. Bertrams A, Englert C. Validierung des STAI-SKD zur Messung von Zustandsangst in echten Prüfungssituationen. Psychol Erzieh Unterr. 2013;60(2):112–20.

    Google Scholar 

  169. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. 

  170. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  171. Klein EM, Brähler E, Dreier M, Reinecke L, Müller KW, Schmutzer G, et al. The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale – psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16(1):159.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  172. Schulz P, Schlotz W. Trierer Inventar zur Erfassung von chronischem Streß (TICS): Skalenkonstruktion, teststatistische Überprüfung und Validierung der Skala Arbeitsüberlastung. Diagnostica. 1999;45(1):8–19.

    Google Scholar 

  173. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  174. Klinitzke G, Romppel M, Häuser W, Brähler E, Glaesmer H. Die deutsche Version des Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) – psychometrische Eigenschaften in einer bevölkerungsrepräsentativen Stichprobe. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2012;62(02):47–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  175. Bless H, Wänke M, Bohner G, Fellhauer RF, Schwarz N. Need for cognition: eine skala zur erfassung von engagement und freude bei denkaufgaben. Z Für Sozialpsychologie. 1994;25:147–54.

    Google Scholar 

  176. Euteneuer F. Subjective social status and health. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27(5):337–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  177. Stewart MB. The interrelated dynamics of unemployment and low-wage employment. J Appl Econom. 2007;22(3):511–31.

    Google Scholar 

  178. Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168(3):242–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  179. Võ MLH, Conrad M, Kuchinke L, Urton K, Hofmann MJ, Jacobs AM. The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(2):534–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  180. Olszanowski M, Pochwatko G, Kuklinski K, Scibor-Rylski M, Lewinski P, Ohme RK. Warsaw set of emotional facial expression pictures: a validation study of facial display photographs. Front Psychol. 2015;5:1516.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  181. Whitmer AJ, Gotlib IH. An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(5):1036–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  182. Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1732–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  183. Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Smith SM. Constrained linear basis sets for HRF modelling using Variational Bayes. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1748–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  184. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith SM. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. Neuroimage. 2001;14(6):1370–86.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  185. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL NeuroImage. 2012;62(2):782–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  186. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:208–19.

    Google Scholar 

  187. Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, et al. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage. 2009;45(1):173–86.

    Google Scholar 

  188. Reid AT, Bzdok D, Langner R, Fox PT, Laird AR, Amunts K, et al. Multimodal connectivity mapping of the human left anterior and posterior lateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221(5):2589–605.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  189. Python Software Foundation. Python. 2023 Available from: https://www.python.org/. [cited 2023 Apr 27].

  190. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn : a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2(3):125–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  191. Posit Software. RStudio. Boston, MA; 2023 Available from: https://www.posit.co/. [cited 2023 May 16].

  192. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2023. Available from: https://www.r-project.org/. [cited 2023 May 16].

  193. McKinney W. pandas: a Foundational Python Library for Data Analysis and Statistics. 2011;

  194. Hunter JD. Matplotlib: A 2D graphics environment. Comput Sci Eng. 2007;9(3):90–5.

    Google Scholar 

  195. Virtanen P, Gommers R, Oliphant TE, Haberland M, Reddy T, Cournapeau D, et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat Methods. 2020;17(3):261–72.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  196. Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2012;11(2):141–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  197. Chavez-Baldini U, Nieman DH, Keestra A, Lok A, Mocking RJT, De Koning P, et al. The relationship between cognitive functioning and psychopathology in patients with psychiatric disorders: a transdiagnostic network analysis. Psychol Med. 2021;24(53):476–85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Clara Walters for her valuable support regarding the bibliography.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This study is funded by the MSB Medical School Berlin. The funding body, represented by the Institutional Review Board of the MSB Medical School Berlin, has reviewed and approved a tabular version of the protocol.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MSM and DW conceptualized the study design, wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and acquire the data. SE and SG substantially revised the study design and edited the manuscript. All authors certify responsibility for this manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marvin S. Meiering.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study is conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the MSB Medical School Berlin (MSB-2022/94). All participants provide written informed consent to participate in this study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Meiering, M.S., Weigner, D., Enge, S. et al. Transdiagnostic phenomena of psychopathology in the context of the RDoC: protocol of a multimodal cross-sectional study. BMC Psychol 11, 297 (2023). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-023-01335-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-023-01335-8

Keywords