Skip to main content

Exploring associations between subjective well-being and personality over a time span of 15–18 months: a cohort study of adolescents in Sweden

Abstract

Introduction

Subjective well-being (SWB) is a contributing factor for building resilience and a resource for positive outcomes, e.g. study achievement and work performance. Earlier studies have examined associations between and prospective effects of personality traits on SWB, but few addressed the role that SWB plays in formation of personality over time. The purpose of our study was to examine associations and prospective effects of SWB on personality traits and vice versa in a cohort sample of secondary school students in Sweden who completed self-reported measures of SWB and personality traits at baseline (N = 446, 76% females) and at 15–18 month follow-up (N = 283, 71% females).

Methods

SWB was defined and measured by the WHO-5 Well-being Index and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. The Big Five Inventory was used to measure personality traits. Autoregressive models were used to analyse associations and potential prospective effects of SWB on personality traits and vice versa.

Results

Low levels of neuroticism and high levels of extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness were associated with high levels of SWB at baseline and follow-up. The association between SWB and neuroticism was notably strong. We found high statistically significant rank order stability across the two time points for all measures of personality traits with stability effects, derived from the autoregressive models, ranging from .199 for extraversion to .440 for neuroticism. Stability for SWB was statistically significant across the two time points and ranged from .182 for well-being to .353 for life satisfaction. SWB had a prospective effect on agreeableness only. None of the personality traits had any significant prospective effects on SWB.

Conclusions

The present findings indicate that although correlated, bidirectional prospective effects between personality traits and SWB could not be confirmed. Neuroticism displayed the strongest negative association with adolescents’ SWB. Schools are an appropriate setting to improve well-being, and allocating resources that reduce neuroticism is crucial, including structural interventions, policies for healthy school settings and teaching emotional regulation techniques.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Most research on mental health remains rooted in deficit models and focuses on mental ill health, e.g. anxiety, depression and stress rather than on well-being, e.g. emotional and psychological functioning and satisfaction with life. This is also true for research on adolescents’ mental health. Although time trends differ regarding an increase and stability across countries in Europe, Israel and North America [1], a clear, however minor increase in mental health complaints is evident for countries in Northern Europe since the 1980s [2]. In Sweden, young people aged 16–29 have reported the highest levels of deteriorated mental health, i.e. symptoms of anxiety, depression, and stress, over the last decade compared to any other age group [3]. The reasons for these heightened levels of self-reported symptoms are yet unclear, but two possible explanations stated in a literature review have gained the most recognition [4]; the first is a deterioration of the Swedish school system, which has undergone several changes in recent decades including a transformation from a centralised public system to a decentralised system and school choice. In parallel, a new school curriculum was introduced with an increased use of assessments, more strict achievement criteria and high-stakes national tests [5]. These reforms might have led to a decline in school performance and increased degrees of internalised problems among children and adolescents. The second possible explanation concerns the more unstable labour market, which has led to higher demands on education and skills. It is probable that young people feel anxiety and stress about not performing well and about their future position in the labour market. However, alongside higher reported levels of mental health complaints, students’ positive mental health in terms of life satisfaction has not declined, but has instead remained relatively stable since 2001/2002 [6]. This may seem contradictory, but it is possible given that positive and negative mental health may be seen as a dual continuum, whereby an individual can experience well-being despite a status of mental ill health [7, 8]. For example, it is not plausible that a mental health status of diagnosed depression could coexist with high life satisfaction, positive affect and happiness, or low negative affect. On the other hand, symptoms of depression may be transient and certainly can co-exist with stable subjective well-being (SWB).

Subjective well-being

The concept SWB, referred to in everyday speech as happiness, peace, fulfilment and life-satisfaction [9], is comprised of emotional well-being in terms of a high degree of positive affect and a low degree of negative affect, as well as high levels of satisfaction with one’s life [10]. SWB has been researched for four decades and has been shown to build resilience as well as being beneficial for health, longevity, work performance and supportive social relationships [11]. During adolescence, a period characterized by rapid developmental changes, a high level of SWB is an important tool to navigate life successfully [12]. A representative study from the United States (US) showed that high levels of life satisfaction and positive affect in adolescence and young adulthood correlated significantly with higher levels of income at about age 29 [13]. In college students, high levels of SWB, albeit not the very highest, were associated with study achievement measured by grade point average [14]. In a cross-sectional study among 7th and 8th grade middle school students in the US, those with the highest levels of SWB were also high study achievers [15]. Intervention studies indicate that SWB may be improved through school-based interventions [16].

In addition to measuring individual well-being, SWB is likewise an indicator of societal well-being and varies across nations and according to gross national income [17, 18]. The link between social capital (specifically the dimensions of trust, social interaction, and norms and sanctions) and the individual happiness aspect of SWB has been investigated in 29 European countries [19]. The authors found that all three dimensions matter for happiness, especially social interaction and general social and institutional trust. However, significant differences were found in how social capital interacts with happiness, and in the Nordic countries only trust was associated with happiness. Diener and colleagues have stated that SWB seems to be a necessary, but not sufficient, characteristic of a good life and a good society, and it should be complemented by economic and social indicators [9].

Associations between personality traits and SWB

Whereas demographic factors such as age and sex are weakly related to SWB, personality traits, together with health and socioeconomic factors, have strong associations according to meta-analyses and reviews [20,21,22,23]. The traits extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience are included in the Big Five Model [24,25,26]. Until the late 1990s, research on personality traits indicated that the mean levels of personality traits change during development in childhood and adolescence, but are principally fixed by age 30 [27]. Later studies rebut these findings and show that changes are possible over the life course and that trait stability continues to increase until after 50 years of age [28], although most changes occur before the age of 40 [29, 30]. Studies on the association between SWB and personality (regardless of study population age) suggest that extraversion is moderately to strongly correlated with positive affect [20, 31,32,33] and high life satisfaction [21]. Research furthermore shows that neuroticism is associated with negative affect [34,35,36] and low life satisfaction [21]. In contrast to previous research a recent meta-analysis shows strong associations between conscientiousness and SWB beyond neuroticism and extraversion [23]. Over a period of one year, a test–retest correlation of approximately 0.76 in the measurement of SWB has been observed [37]. Furthermore, over a period of four weeks, a test–retest correlation of approximately 0.88 in the measurement of Big Five personality traits was found [38].

The association between personality and SWB has been mostly studied in adult populations or among young adults [18, 20, 39]. In adolescents, the associations have mainly been assessed in cross-sectional studies without predictive possibilities. In studies on adolescents in corresponding age ranges with our population and conducted in a Western context, neuroticism (low levels), extraversion, and conscientiousness seem to be the strongest factors associated with SWB. This is also apparent in studies on adolescents from Sweden [40], and from the US [41, 42], whereas other studies have only identified significant associations between neuroticism (negatively) and extraversion, and SWB [43, 44] in Swedish and American samples. Nonetheless, a longitudinal study in adolescents on vocational education and training shows another pattern [45]. In this German cohort with a mean age of 18 years, agreeableness and conscientiousness exhibited the strongest associations with life satisfaction at a follow-up measurement of 15 months as well as after three years.

Until recently a common assumption was that personality is relatively stable and causes well-being, but not the reverse [20, 21, 27]. This postulation has been challenged in a large national representative Australian sample of persons aged 15–93 [35]; Soto found that higher levels of extraversion, agreeableness and conscientiousness and emotional stability predicted subsequent levels of SWB. Additionally, high levels of initial well-being predicted agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, i.e. the counterpart of neuroticism, and introversion, i.e. the counterpart of extraversion, at a later point in time. However, it should be noted that although many of the prospective associations in Soto’s study were significant, due to a large sample (N = 16,367) they were weak, with only one standardized association exceeding 0.1 (0.137). In the present study we intended to investigate if Soto’s findings are maintained when examined in a younger age group, namely adolescents between the ages of 16 and 18. In summary, several scholars before us have longitudinally examined the correlation between personality traits and their effects on SWB over time. Nonetheless, whether SWB may also predict personality has to our knowledge only been investigated and confirmed by Specht [46] and Soto [35] in their studies on predominantly adult populations.

Purpose of the study

The purpose of this longitudinal study was to analyse associations and prospective effects of SWB on personality traits, and vice versa, over 15–18 month follow up in a cohort sample of girls and boys in secondary schools in Sweden.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The sample (N = 446, age range 16–18 years, 76% females) at baseline consisted of students in secondary schools in Sweden. Among them, 283 adolescents (71% females) answered the questionnaire at the 15–18-month follow-up. At baseline and follow-up the questionnaires were filled out on a voluntary basis during school lessons and school staff collected the sealed envelopes. The sample was recruited from four secondary schools in middle and south of Sweden. The schools provided academic oriented programs, and the entrance requirements were among the highest in the country. Originally, the recruitment aimed at participation in two school-based intervention studies with controls, with the intention to prevent stress and symptoms of depression. One of the interventions was directed only towards girls, and followed up at 15 months. The other intervention was followed up at 18 months after baseline measurements. All measurements were administered before the COVID-19 pandemic. As the conducted interventions showed no effects on either SWB or personality traits at the follow-up, we merged the intervention and control groups in order to gain a larger sample for the present cohort study. The study has been approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of the Stockholm Committee (No. 2009/1788–31/3), and informed consent was obtained from the participants.

Measurements

Subjective well-being (SWB)

SWB was defined and measured by two scales: a) the WHO-5 Well-being Index (WHO-5) and b) the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS).

WHO-5 well-being index (WHO-5)

Well-being was measured by the World Health Organization Well-being Index (WHO-5). A systematic review of its use in a range of countries and populations, including adolescents, concluded that the WHO-5 tapped into the SWB of the respondents and that the scale is suitable for research on well-being over time [47]. The WHO-5 contains five statements (e.g. I have felt cheerful and in good spirits), which are rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 5 = all of the time to 0 = at no time, and the respondent is asked to reflect on the last two weeks. The raw scores ranging from 0 to 25 are multiplied by 4, where 100 indicates best imaginable well-being. The WHO-5 Well-being Index was translated into Swedish and validated in an adult population sample aged 19 to 64 [48]. The alpha reliabilities of the scale were 0.81 at baseline and 0.67 at follow-up.

Satisfaction with life scale (SWLS)

SWB does not only include people’s emotional responses to ongoing life, but also the evaluative factor of life satisfaction [49]. The SWLS has been used in various countries and among different populations, including Swedish adolescents [50]. According to Topp et al. (2015) the WHO-5 is a sufficient instrument for measuring SWB, yet, as it only includes a single item on life satisfaction we also included the SWLS [51] in our measurement of SWB. Another argument to include the SWLS was the finding by Specht et al. that positive changes in life satisfaction were prospectively associated with positive changes in personality traits [46].

In this 5-item inventory, the respondent is asked to rate his or her agreement with statements (e.g. I am satisfied with my life) on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. The items are scored by summing up the raw scores ranging from 5 to 35. The scale has been translated into Swedish and used previously in an adolescent sample [52]. The scale’s alpha reliabilities were 0.82 at baseline and 0.86 at follow-up.

Big five inventory (BFI)

The BFI is frequently used to measure personality traits in different countries and populations, including adolescents [53,54,55]. The personality traits were assessed with the 44-item version by John, Naumann and Soto [26]. The instrument measures extraversion (8 items), agreeableness (9 items), conscientiousness (9 items), neuroticism (8 items), and openness (10 items). All items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree, to 5 = strongly agree. Scoring instructions were provided by the Berkeley Personality Lab, Institute of Personality & Social Research. The Swedish version of the BFI was used in this study. The instrument has been validated in a Swedish context [56] and in another study involving a sample of adolescents, it was translated from English into Swedish and then back-translated, and no significant discrepancies were found [43]. The scale’s alpha reliabilities at baseline and 15–18 month follow-up were 0.83 and 0.85 for extraversion, 0.73 and 0.69 for agreeableness, 0.81 and 0.81 for conscientiousness, 0.81 and 0.83 for neuroticism, and 0.74 and 0.75 for openness, respectively.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics means (M) and standard deviations (SD) were used to outline the distribution of personality traits and SWB at baseline and at 15–18-month follow-up. As SWB and personality traits were measured at only two time points, autoregressive models were used to analyse associations between as well as prospective effects of SWB on the Big Five personality traits and vice-versa. All items of the BFI, the WHO-5, and the SWLS were included in the autoregressive models. As missing values could not be assumed to be missing completely at random (MCAR) listwise deletion was not advisable. Therefore, through multiple imputation, twenty datasets were created in which the missing values were replaced by predicted values. These predicted values varied between the datasets, reflecting uncertainty in such predictions. The parameter values presented below in the autoregressive models are pooled means calculated across the twenty datasets. A sensitivity analysis was conducted by calculating the associations in models employing full information maximum likelihood (FIML) rather than imputations.

Following Soto [35], autoregressive models with cross-lagged effects between Big Five personality traits and measures of SWB were fitted to data (Fig. 1). For example, the latent variables extraversion (with eight manifest indicators/items) and SWB (five indicators/items), measured at 15–18 month follow-up, were regressed on extraversion and SWB measured at baseline. The five measures of personality and two measures of SWB resulted in ten analysed models. The error terms of the same item, e.g. item three on the extraversion scale, were allowed to correlate with each other across the two time points. According to Soto, such latent autoregressive models are good at evaluating prospective influences, for example between personality traits and well-being.

Fig.1
figure 1

Illustration of the analysed autoregressive models including measures of SWB (measured by the WHO-5 well-being index, and the satisfaction with life scale) and personality traits (measured by the big five inventory) at baseline and at 15–18-month follow-up. The arrows indicate: a = SWB Prospective Effects, i.e. effects of SWB at baseline on Trait at follow-up; b = Trait Stability, i.e. effects of Trait at baseline on Trait at follow-up; c = Trait Prospective Effects, i.e. effects of Trait at baseline on SWB at follow-up; d = SWB Stability, i.e. effects of SWB at baseline on SWB at follow-up; e = Associations between SWB and Trait at baseline; f = Associations between SWB and Trait at follow-up. The error terms for the same item (e.g. T3) were allowed to correlate across the two time points. For measurement invariance, factor loadings (e.g. t3), intercepts, and residuals of the same item were constrained to be equal at the two time points. Parameters b and d indicate rank-order stability and are not affected by possible mean level changes in SWB/personality traits

In order to achieve measurement invariance, i.e. that the latent variables were comparable measures of the same constructs at the two time points, several constrictions were added to the models. First, the factor loadings of the same item (e.g. item three of extraversion, see parameter t3 in Fig. 1) were constrained to be equal at the two time points. Second, the intercepts of the same item were constrained to be equal at the two time points. Together with the constriction of factor loadings, this means that for the same level of a latent variable, e.g. extraversion, the score on the same manifest indicator, e.g. item three on the extraversion scale, was predicted to be the same at the two time points. Third, the residuals of the same item were constrained to be equal at the two time points. This means that the same amount of variance in a manifest indicator, e.g. item 3 on the extraversion scale, was assumed to be accounted for by variance in the latent variable, in this case extraversion, at the two time points. Without measurement invariance an observed difference between time points could be due to a change in the meaning of the construct rather than in its general level.

Analyses were conducted with R.3.5.0 statistical software [57], employing the packages lavaan [58], semTools [59], and Amelia [60].

Results

Personality traits and SWB at baseline and at 15–18-month follow-up

Descriptive statistics of the sample and included measurements at baseline and 15–18-month follow-up are shown in Table 1. The cohort sample consisted of N = 446, 76% females, at baseline, and N = 283, 71% females at follow-up. No significant baseline differences were found between study completers and study dropouts on either the Big Five personality traits or the WHO-5 and SWLS. However, there was a significantly higher attrition rate among girls than among boys. Descriptive statistics for all items and correlations between all individual items as well as between composite scores are presented in the Additional file 1: Table S1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the sample and included measurements

Standardized parameter values for the ten autoregressive models are presented in Table 2. Associations were obvious between personality traits and SWB when measured at the same time, negative for neuroticism and positive for the others and generally non-significant for openness. There was statistically significant rank order stability across the two time points for the measures of personality traits, with stability effects ranging from 0.199 for extraversion to 0.440 for neuroticism. Similarly, the stability for SWB, i.e. well-being and life satisfaction, was statistically significant across the two time points and ranged from 0.182 for wellbeing to 0.353 for life satisfaction. None of the five trait factors showed any significant prospective effects on well-being and life satisfaction. The only significant cross-lagged effect, 0.203, was shown for well-being on agreeableness. These findings were verified by analyses employing full information likelihood (FIML) estimations rather than imputations (Additional file 2: Table S2).

Table 2 Autoregressive standardized effects and correlations with standard error between personality traits and subjective well-being

Discussion

Main findings and comparisons to other studies

The main finding in this cohort study is that low levels of neuroticism and high levels of, extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness are associated with high levels of well-being and life satisfaction in our sample when measured simultaneously at baseline, as well as at 15–18 month follow-up.

The principal contribution of the study was our attempt to test not only if personality traits had prospective effects on SWB, but also if SWB had prospective effects on personality. To our knowledge, the latter test has only been conducted among adults in two different studies [35, 46] but not in a sample of adolescents in secondary education. Our study was not able to replicate Soto’s [35] main findings, namely that SWB may over time predict personality traits in a similar way as personality traits prospectively predict well-being. None of the five personality traits showed any prospective effect on SWB, i.e. well-being measured by the WHO-5 and by the SWLS. However, as stated in the introduction, Soto [35] analysed data from a much larger sample (N = 16, 367) compared to the present study. Consequently, the difference in result could mainly be due to a difference in power rather than a difference in the strength of the associations. The strongest prospective effect of SWB on personality found by Soto [35] was -0.056 and of personality on SWB 0.137, i.e. not very different from the associations in the present study.

Our main finding, the association between the four mentioned personality traits and well-being and life satisfaction in the order as mentioned above., confirms earlier studies in adolescent populations living in high-income countries [40, 42, 43, 61]. For example, in two other Swedish samples of high-school students, albeit cross-sectional studies, neuroticism showed the strongest negative association with SWB [40, 43], followed by conscientiousness and extraversion, respectively. A study involving a sample of Norwegian folk high school students with a mean age of 19 years also found neuroticism (measured as emotional stability) to be more strongly associated with SWB compared to extraversion [61]. In a US sample of high school students, an instrument consistent with the Big Five Model was tested with life satisfaction as outcome. The analyses revealed that 47% of the variance in adolescents’ life satisfaction was related to personality traits and neuroticism (negatively) turned out to be the strongest predictor, -0.72, followed by conscientiousness 0.18, extraversion 0.14, and openness 0.13 [42]. Similar to our study, none of the abovementioned comparative studies showed strong correlations with agreeableness.

The observed test–retest correlation in SWB in the present study was weaker than in a study by Anglim et al., approximately 0.30 versus 0.76 [37]. The difference could, for example, be due to differences in age between the two samples. Further, in our study the test–retest correlation in personality traits was weaker than in a study by Wood et al., approximately 0.35 versus 0.88 [38]. The difference could be due to differences in time between the measurements.

The studies show similarities as well as differences in the results. Potential explanations for these differences include the specific characteristics of the study population, the context where the studies are conducted in, the time frame when the measurement have been undertaken, the study design, and other factors out of control.

Limitations, strengths, and future research

A lower proportion of boys than girls participated in this study at both baseline and follow-up, which resulted in an uneven group size of boys and girls. The low proportion of boys in our sample limited us from performing an analysis stratified by gender. A larger population sample and hence an increased statistical power might result in more significant effects.

Other limitations are the relatively short follow-up time of 15–18 months, and the absence of further waves of follow-up. A longer time interval between the assessments might have resulted in a personality maturation and higher levels of change over time in personality and aspects of well-being, as shown in other studies [46, 62]. Additional waves of follow-up could have given a more comprehensive picture of the development in stability of traits and SWB.

We and other scholars we refer to, analysed the association and possible prospective effects between personality and SWB. Although SWB is an important factor and measures life satisfaction and hedonic well-being, i.e. brief happiness, it is not sufficient for a complete assessment of positive mental health. To give a more extensive picture of the complexity in well-being, SWB should be complemented by a measurement on eudaimonic well-being, i.e. self- realization and psychological functioning [63]. An adequate measurement may be Carol Ryff’s instrument, which encompasses six constructs of well-being: autonomy, personal growth, self-acceptance, life purpose, mastery, and the ability to have positive relations with others [64]. These constructs have been identified as important factors in being able to cope with challenges in life [65].

Our cohort sample consisted of high-achieving girls and boys in secondary schools with good reputations and the results might therefore be generalisable only to adolescents in corresponding schools. The Swedish school system has been undergoing a structural change since the 1990s, involving the establishment of independent schools and increased possibilities to choose between different schools. In parallel with the rise of independent schools, performance gaps between schools have widened, and increased variations in grades between schools have been shown [66]. The results of our study may therefore be generalisable to those secondary schools with high performance students. A replication of this study with a more representative and diverse study-population would strengthen the results. The inclusion of a measurement on eudaimonic well-being in addition to SWB would also provide a more comprehensive picture of the concept of well-being.

The relative lack of power compared to Soto’s study [35] with a larger sample, has already been mentioned. This increases the risk for missing associations that actually exist, especially if they are weak. On the other hand, as the present study employed more than one test of significance, the risk for type 1 errors, i.e. observing a significant association purely due to chance, is inflated above the nominal 5%. As we analysed ten different models, a conservative Bonferroni correction would require us to use 0.05/10 = 0.005 as the level of significance and this would mean that associations with one or two stars in Table 2 would not be considered significant. However, if we take into consideration that each of these models included six different parameters (not counting the factor loadings), the used significance level would be even lower at 0.05/ (10 × 6) = 0.0008. These corrections would decrease the power of the study, and increase the risk for type 2 errors, even further. Thus, our data should be interpreted with caution. We would suggest a replication of our study with a larger sample to explore if prospective effects of well-being on, for instance, agreeableness would be found.

Conclusions

The present findings indicate that although correlated, bidirectional prospective effects between personality traits and SWB as found by Soto and colleagues could not be confirmed, and thus may not be a universal phenomenon. However, low power in the present study makes conclusions about lack of effects uncertain. Neuroticism is the trait displaying the strongest negative association with adolescents’ SWB. Allocating resources that reduce neuroticism and elevate well-being in schools is a crucial issue. This could be accomplished through structural interventions and policies that create healthy school settings and school-based health promotion including emotional learning techniques.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are not publicly available, as it requires ethical approval to use the datasets according to Swedish legislation. However, the datasets are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Ottova-Jordan V, Smith OR, Gobina I, Mazur J, Augustine L, Cavallo F, et al. Trends in multiple recurrent health complaints in 15-year-olds in 35 countries in Europe, North America and Israel from 1994 to 2010. Eur J Public Health. 2015;25(Suppl 2):24–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/eurpub/ckv015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Potrebny T, Wiium N, Lundegard MM. Temporal trends in adolescents’ self-reported psychosomatic health complaints from 1980–2016: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11): e0188374. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0188374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Public Health Agency of Sweden: Statistik över vuxnas psykiska hälsa [Statistics on mental health in adults]. https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/livsvillkor-levnadsvanor/psykisk-halsa-och-suicidprevention/statistik-psykisk-halsa/vuxnas-psykiska-halsa/. 2019; Accessed 15 Mar 2019.

  4. Public Health Agency of Sweden: Why have multiple health complaints increased among children and adolescents in Sweden? The development between 1985 and 2014. https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/contentassets/628f1bfc932b474f9503cc6f8e29fd45/varfor-psykiska-ohalsan-okat-barn-unga-18023-2-webb-rapport.pdf. 2018; Accessed 2019 10 Feb 2019.

  5. Högberg B, Lindgren J, Johansson K, Strandh M, Petersen S. Consequences of school grading systems on adolescent health: evidence from a Swedish school reform. J Educ Policy. 2021;36(1):84–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Public Health Agency of Sweden. Health behaviour of school-aged children (HBSC), results from Sweden of the 2017/18 WHO study. Solna and Östersund 2019, p. 144.

  7. Keyes C. The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43(2):207–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Westerhof GJ, Keyes CL. Mental illness and mental health: the two continua model across the lifespan. J Adult Dev. 2010;17(2):110–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10804-009-9082-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Diener E, Oishi S, Lucas RE. Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:403–25. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. 2008;9(1):1–11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Diener E, Seligman MEP, Choi H, Oishi S. Happiest People Revisited. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2018;13(2):176–84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1745691617697077.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Keyes CLM. The nature and importance of positive mental health in America’s adolescents. In: Furlong MJ, Gilman R, Huebner ES, editors. Handbook of positive psychology in schools. New York: Taylor and Francis; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  13. De Neve JE, Oswald AJ. Estimating the influence of life satisfaction and positive affect on later income using sibling fixed effects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(49):19953–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.1211437109.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Oishi S, Diener E, Lucas RE. The optimum level of well-being: can people be too happy? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(4):346–60. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00048.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Whitley A, Huebner E, Hills K, Valois R. Can students be too happy in school? The optimal level of school satisfaction. Appl Res Qual Life. 2012;7(4):337–50. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11482-012-9167-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. O’Connor C, Dyson J, Cowdell F, Watson R. Do universal school-based mental health promotion programmes improve the mental health and emotional well-being of young people? A literature review. J Clin Nurs. 2017. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/jocn.14078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Diener E. New findings and future directions for subjective well-being research. Am Psychol. 2012;67(8):590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Cheng C, Cheung MW, Montasem A. Explaining differences in subjective well-being across 33 nations using multilevel models: universal personality, cultural relativity, and national income. J Pers. 2016;84(1):46–58. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/jopy.12136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Rodríguez-Pose A, von Berlepsch V. Social capital and individual happiness in Europe. J Happiness Stud. 2014;15(2):357–86. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10902-013-9426-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. De Neve K, Cooper H. The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(2):197–229. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(1):138–61. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. John Hopkins University: Covid-19 Dashboard by the Center for Systems Science and Engineering (CSSE) https://gisanddata.maps.arcgis.com/apps/opsdashboard/index.html#/bda7594740fd40299423467b48e9ecf6. 2020; Accessed 24 Jun 2020.

  23. Anglim J, Horwood S, Smillie LD, Marrero RJ, Wood JK. Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2020;146(4):279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Costa PTJ, McCrae RR. Neo personality inventory–revised (neo-pi-r) and neo five-factor inventory (neo-ffi) professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Goldberg LR. The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychol Assess. 1992;4(1):26–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative big five trait taxonomy. In: John OP, Roberts RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: theory and research, vol. 2. New York: Guildford Press; 2008. p. 114–58.

    Google Scholar 

  27. McCrae RR, Costa PT. The stability of personality: observations and evaluations. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1994;3(6):173–5. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2000;126(1):3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Robins RW, Fraley RC, Roberts BW, Trzesniewski KH. A longitudinal study of personality change in young adulthood. J Pers. 2001;69(4):617–40. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/1467-6494.694157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(1):1–25. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Diener E, Lucas RE. Personality and subjective well-being. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1999. p. 213–29.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Lucas RE, Fujita F. Factors influencing the relation between extraversion and pleasant affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;79(6):1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Lucas RE, Le K, Dyrenforth PS. Explaining the extraversion/positive affect relation: sociability cannot account for extraverts’ greater happiness. J Pers. 2008;76(3):385–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rusting CL, Larsen RJ. Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: a test of two theoretical models. Person Individ Differ. 1997;22(5):607–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Soto CJ. Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the big five with subjective well-being. J Pers. 2015;83(1):45–55. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/jopy.12081.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Lucas RE. Personality and subjective well-being. In: Eid M, Larsen RJ, editors. The science of subjective well-being. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. p. 171–94.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Anglim J, Weinberg MK, Cummins RA. Bayesian hierarchical modeling of the temporal dynamics of subjective well-being: a 10 year longitudinal analysis. J Res Pers. 2015;59:1–14. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Wood JK, Anglim J, Horwood S. A less evaluative measure of big five personality: comparison of structure and criterion validity. Eur J Person. 2021

  39. Friedman HS, Kern ML, Reynolds CA. Personality and health, subjective well-being, and longevity. J Pers. 2010;78(1):179–216. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00613.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Garcia D. Two models of personality and well-being among adolescents. Person Individ Differ. 2011;50(8):1208–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Hudson NW, Roberts BW. Goals to change personality traits: concurrent links between personality traits, daily behavior, and goals to change oneself. J Res Pers. 2014;53:68–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Suldo SM, Minch DR, Hearon BV. Adolescent life satisfaction and personality characteristics: investigating relationships using a five factor model. J Happiness Stud. 2015;16(4):965–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kjell ON, Nima AA, Sikström S, Archer T, Garcia D. Iranian and Swedish adolescents: differences in personality traits and well-being. PeerJ. 2013;1:e197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Kim SY, Chang MY, Hong M, Yoo SG, Oh D, Park MK. Tinnitus therapy using tailor-made notched music delivered via a smartphone application and Ginko combined treatment: a pilot study. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2017;44(5):528–33. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.anl.2016.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Deventer J, Lüdtke O, Nagy G, Retelsdorf J, Wagner J. Against all odds–is a more differentiated view of personality development in emerging adulthood needed? The case of young apprentices. Br J Psychol. 2019;110(1):60–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC. Examining mechanisms of personality maturation: the impact of life satisfaction on the development of the big five personality traits. Soc Psychol Person Sci. 2013;4(2):181–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1948550612448197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Topp CW, Ostergaard SD, Sondergaard S, Bech P. The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167–76. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1159/000376585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Love J, Andersson L, Moore CD, Hensing G. Psychometric analysis of the Swedish translation of the WHO well-being index. Qual Life Res. 2014;23(1):293–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11136-013-0447-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Diener E, Lucas RE, Oishi S. Advances and open questions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra Psychol. 2018. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1525/collabra.115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Garcia D, Moradi S. The affective temperaments and well-being: Swedish and Iranian adolescents’ life satisfaction and psychological well-being. J Happiness Stud. 2013;14(2):689–707. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10902-012-9349-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49(1):71–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Garcia D, Siddiqui A. Adolescents’ affective temperaments: life satisfaction, interpretation, and memory of events. J Positive Psychol. 2009;4(2):155–67. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/17439760802399349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Kulig TC, Cullen FT, Wilcox P, Chouhy C. Personality and adolescent school-based victimization: do the big five matter? J Sch Violence. 2019;18(2):176–99. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/15388220.2018.1444495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Borghuis J, Denissen JJA, Oberski D, Sijtsma K, Meeus WHJ, Branje S, et al. Big five personality stability, change, and codevelopment across adolescence and early adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017;113(4):641–57. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/pspp0000138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Branje SJ, Van Lieshout CF, Gerris JR. Big Five personality development in adolescence and adulthood. Eur J Pers. 2007;21(1):45–62. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/per.596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Zakrisson I. Big Five Inventory (BFI): Utprövning för svenska förhållanden. Mid Sweden University; 2010.

  57. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. https://www.r-project.org/. 2018; Accessed 3 Mar 2019.

  58. Rosseel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2).

  59. Jorgensen T, Pornprasertmanit S, Schoemann A, Rosseel Y, Miller P, Quick C et al. semTools: useful tools for structural equation modeling. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semTools. 2018; Accessed 3 Mar 2019.

  60. Honaker J, King G, Blackwell M. Amelia II: a program for missing data. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(7):1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Vittersø J. Personality traits and subjective well-being: emotional stability, not extraversion, is probably the important predictor. Person Individ Differ. 2001;31(6):903–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Scollon CN, Diener E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;91(6):1152–65. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ryan R, Deci E. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52(1):141–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J Person Soc Psychol. 1989. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Ryff CD. Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother Psychosom. 2014;83(1):10–28. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1159/000353263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Östh J, Andersson E, Malmberg B. School choice and increasing performance difference: a counterfactual approach. Urban Stud. 2013;50(2):407–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to all students who participated in this study.

Funding

Open access funding has been provided by Karolinska Institutet.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RW designed and conceptualized the study. KS performed the statistical analyses and RW, MV and LL reviewed the analyses. RW drafted the initial version of the manuscript, and all authors contributed to revising, editing, and finalizing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Regina Winzer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study has been approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of the Stockholm Committee (No. 2009/1788-31/3), and informed consent was obtained from the participants. Parental consent from parents for adolescents ≥ 15 years is not required, due to Swedish legislation on ethical review of research on human beings (SFS 2003:460, Section 18). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. All methods have been carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Full correlation matrix, correlation composite scores, and item information.

Additional file 2

. Table S2 Autoregressive standardized effects and correlations (with SE) between personality traits and subjective well-being (SWB). Analyses conducted with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Winzer, R., Vaez, M., Lindberg, L. et al. Exploring associations between subjective well-being and personality over a time span of 15–18 months: a cohort study of adolescents in Sweden. BMC Psychol 9, 173 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-021-00673-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-021-00673-9

Keywords