Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Does a lower self-concept contribute to mental health disparities of diverse immigrant youth from middle childhood to late adolescence?

Abstract

Background

Three out of ten children in Germany have immigrant backgrounds and this proportion is expected to further increase in subsequent years. While immigrant youth have been found more vulnerable to developing symptoms of depression and anxiety, the underlying mechanisms of how such disparities unfold during youth development are still understudied. Some previous research has found that immigrant youth are at risk of experiencing a less positive self-concept compared to non-immigrant youth. We investigated whether the self-concept mediates mental health disparities and explored variability in such associations from middle childhood to late adolescence.

Methods

Overall 1839 children and adolescents aged 6–21 years (M = 14.05 years, SD = 3.03, 49.8% female, n = 782 with immigrant status) participated in a cross-sectional self-report survey in classroom settings using scales from the Beck Youth Inventories II (Beck et al. in Beck Youth Inventories - Second Edition, Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, 2005) to assess self-concept and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Links between immigrant status, age, self-concept and symptom levels of depression as well as anxiety were examined using hierarchical regression and moderated mediation models.

Results

Immigrant youth reported higher symptom levels of depression and anxiety than their non-immigrant peers but did not differ in their self-concepts. Hypothesized moderated mediation models were not fully supported and self-concept neither mediated the link between immigrant status and depression nor immigrant status and anxiety. However, self-concept was a significant predictor for symptom levels of depression as well as anxiety, with stronger associations in adolescents.

Conclusions

Our study substantiates previous findings that immigrant youth in Germany have overall increased symptom levels of depression and anxiety compared to non-immigrant youth. Our study however does not support that immigrant youth have a more negative self-concept and that the self-concept mediates such internalizing mental health disparities. Findings match previous evidence that developing a positive attitude towards the self is linked to better mental health. Beyond that, our findings suggest that mental health interventions addressing the self-concept could be especially relevant when targeting adolescents. Further research is needed to deepen the understanding of the mediating processes between migration status and mental health variables.

Peer Review reports

Background

About one-fifth of all children and adolescents in Germany experience mental health problems [2, 3] with symptoms of depression and anxiety being among the most common in these age groups [4]. Evidence is alarming, as developing a clinically relevant anxiety or depressive disorder during childhood and adolescence impairs the unfolding of developmental potentials and increases the risk for mental health problems during adulthood [2]. Notably, a higher risk for developing such common internalizing mental health problems has been reported for diverse groups of children and adolescents from immigrant families residing in European countries [5, 6] including newly arrived refugees [7] and those, whose families have lived in the resettlement country for the second or even third generation [8]. Symptoms associated with depression and anxiety have been consistently linked to severe psychological and social consequences among immigrant youth; Plener et al. [9] found a higher prevalence of suicide attempts compared to their non-immigrant counterparts, van Oort et al. [10] substantiated links to the emergence of socio-economic disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant populations during adulthood.

With a growing number of children and adolescents in Germany, currently 36%, have an immigrant status [11], research on the foundations of systematic differences in the mental health status between immigrant and non-immigrant youth is becoming increasingly relevant. Specifically, a better understanding of distinctive mechanisms is an important step to mitigate mental health disparities within the diverse societies of tomorrow. A literature review including twenty global studies on immigrant youth in resettlement countries [12] suggested a lower socio-economic status as a mediator for increased levels of mental health problems among young immigrant populations. However, the reviewed studies also supported that the relations between holding an immigrant background and experiencing mental health problems often persisted above and beyond the explanatory effects of socio-economic status [12]. Some of the reviewed evidence demonstrated within-family stress between first- and second-generation immigrants to additionally account for mental health problems in immigrant youth. Comparing the reviewed studies on within-family conflicts between first- and second-generation immigrants yielded that such findings could be at least partially confounded with age [13]. When immigrant youth are compared to non-immigrant youth of the same age group disparities seem to diminish [14]. Within-family stress is thus more likely related to a specific developmental period and the specific effects linked to immigrant background still remain to be delineated further. Previous findings on the mediating processes were moreover critically discussed as studies were conducted in different and also heterogenous populations, applied varying methodology and, even partially, yielded inconsistent evidence. For example, both lower and higher levels of mental health problems in immigrant youth when compared to non-immigrant youth were found in some studies [15, 16]. One methodological critique addressed the absence of univocal definitions of key constructs such as “immigrant background” or the measurements for mental health problems in immigrant youth [12].

Another recent literature review distinctively studied potential mechanisms and discussed pathways of how mental health disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant populations could unfold [17]. Determinants such as language proficiency, sex, age and, again, socio-economic status were found to be linked to differences in health literacy among immigrant and non-immigrant populations that subsequently are linked to mental health disparities. Authors of that review additionally discussed how heightened access barriers within healthcare systems disadvantage immigrant populations (i.e., less health care benefits/ less opportunities to develop health literacy) and thus likely contribute to mental health disparities [17]. Other studies adopting ecocultural perspectives found cultural distance to the host community [18, 19], social stress [6] and acculturative stress [20] to be linked to increased levels of especially internalizing mental health problems among immigrant youth.

Previous studies on the determinants and mechanisms of mental health disparities in immigrant youth so far have less focused on the question of how those disparities unfold within-person and have seldom considered differences across developmental periods. To shed light on such genuine developmental psychopathological processes, this study investigates the self-concept as a correlate for internalizing mental health problems during youth development. Self-concept is defined as the overall system of beliefs, cognitive and affective attitudes towards oneself. The term summarizes a person’s self-evaluation regarding specific aspects such as one’s academic abilities, physical performance or self-regulation capacities [21, 22]. The self-concept constitutes intrinsic cognitive schemata mainly determined by feedback from social environments [23]. Self-directed feedback is processed intra-personally in order to form a coherent picture of oneself. Previous research [24] has described two strategies of how the self-concept can be structurally organized, namely compartmentalization and integration. Compartmentalization describes a self-image that is structured in different categories containing either positive or negative components of oneself, while integration forms a self-concept that consists of positive as well as negative information about the self [24]. Evidence that links the structure of the self-concept to self-esteem suggests that individuals who tend to compartmentalize their self-concept may experience difficulties in accessing coherent information about oneself, leading to an overall more negative evaluation of the self [25].

A negative and unstable self-concept has also been linked to symptoms of anxiety and depression among children and adolescents in general populations [2630]. For example, a self-report survey with children aged 9 to 13 years in school settings showed that those with symptoms of depression and anxiety also reported lower levels of global self-esteem and a lower quality of life. Such links were stronger in older children of the sample [31]. However, no study has yet focused on the self-concept’s distinctive significance for internalizing mental health problems among immigrant youth. Some previous evidence supports the notion that immigrant children and adolescents are more vulnerable to a lower self-concept [32]— although such evidence is still limited and remains inconsistent. Conceptually, immigrant status is a critical biographical characteristic for children and adolescents as it is linked to intra- and interindividual processes of adaptation and to an increased risk of experiencing stigmatization [33]. Even for those immigrant children and adolescents without personal migration experiences, their origin culture as conveyed by relatives and the migration experience of parents or grandparents still forms the children’s and adolescents’ self-ascriptions [33, 34]. Socio-cultural discontinuities that immigrant youth likely experience between the family context and the host country’s society can impede the formation of a positive and integrative self-concept throughout youth development [35, 38, 39]. While immigrant children and adolescents are exposed to the host countries’ influences in school contexts and peer-networks, they are often taught the language, traditions and values of their origin country at home [36]. Such potentially opposing contexts can result in the development of a fragmented "self-culture" whose principles are ambiguous, context-dependent, conflicting and which can be accompanied by symptoms of withdrawal and self-insecurity [8, 37]. The fragmentation could thus promote a compartmentalized structure of the self-concept.

Adding a developmental perspective, an inner focus on the emerging self-concept arises with the beginning of puberty and peaks during adolescence [38]. Adolescence marks a hypersensitive period to all kinds of social stimuli when a coherent self-concept is formed via information seeking [40, 41]. During that period, schools provide a central social context to establish a coherent self-concept. Since immigrant youth are at heightened risk to experience cultural discontinuities between school and non-school contexts, forming a coherent and positive self-concept in schools could be particularly challenging for this group [42, 43]. Reviewing the research on immigrant children and adolescents from different generations in school-contexts, previous work has mainly focused on the academic components of the self-concept. For example, Mitchell [44] overall found a lower academic self-concept for immigrant adolescents with Central American backgrounds attending schools in the United States and substantiated links to lower levels of well-being. The author, however, did not find differences regarding the generational status of immigration (i.e., first-, second- and third-generation). In further studies a lower self-concept was correspondingly considered a socially-determined and multi-directional pathogenic influence [45, 46], also impacting internalizing mental health problems [47]. It is therefore necessary to extend research on the self-concept among immigrant youth to self-directed cognitions and emotions within school settings.

Investigating the role of the self-concept throughout youth development, our study goal was to better understand the foundations of symptoms of depression and anxiety in immigrant children and adolescents. Specifically, our study hypotheses were to replicate previous findings that (H1) immigrant children and adolescents experience higher symptom levels of depression and anxiety than non-immigrants in school settings, and (H2) that a lower self-concept was linked to those increased symptom levels. Based on this replication, we expected (H3) the self-concept to mediate the respective links between immigrant status and depression as well as anxiety, and (H4) that the effects on symptom levels are stronger during adolescence. The theoretical model reflecting the study hypotheses is illustrated in Fig. 1a.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Panel a illustrates the moderated mediation model according to our study hypotheses. We proposed that immigrant youth report increased symptom levels of depression and anxiety (H1), lower self-concept predicts higher symptom levels (H2), self-concept mediates the link between immigrant status and symptom levels (H3) and that age moderates the hypothesized mediation (H4). Panel b illustrates the statistical realization as suggested by Hayes [56] and corresponds to the analyses reported in Table 6

Methods

Study design and sample

Overall, N = 1907 children and adolescents participated in a cross-sectional self-report survey that we conducted in primary and secondary schools across Germany’s most populous and multicultural federal state, North-Rhine Westphalia. We chose schools in different regions (i.e., varying the degree of urbanization) and neighbourhoods and considered all types of German secondary schools to balance the student sample. At study enrollment, teachers informed parents using written handouts in several languages. Parents who agreed to letting their children participate in the study signed an informed consent. All students in primary and secondary school classrooms were eligible to participate if they were regular members of the class, had a minimum of second grade reading comprehension and presented their parental declaration of consent. We intended to classify our full study sample post-hoc into a non-immigrant and an immigrant group based on their own or familial history of migration. Data were collected via classroom surveys according to a standardized administration protocol. No more than 8 participants per study facilitator were assessed at once in secondary schools. For primary school children, group surveys with a maximum of 4 children per study facilitator were conducted to increase levels of assistance, when necessary. Due to difficulties in the recruitment of primary schools, additional data collections were conducted in sports and social clubs for younger children following the same procedure. All study facilitators were advanced undergraduate or graduate students in psychology, supervised by clinically experienced researchers.

Measures

The study questionnaire comprised a demographic section including participants’ age, sex, migration status and the indicators for parents’ socio-economic status. The highest number of parental school years and parents’ profession according to the International Socio-Economic Index of Occupational Status [48] were used to assess the familial socio-economic status of the sample. To measure participants’ internalizing mental health problems, subscales of the German version of the Beck Youth Inventories-II (BYI-II) were used [49]. The BYI-II is a set of overall 5 self-report questionnaires with 20 items each for assessing depression, anxiety, self-concept, anger and disruptive behaviour in children and adolescents aged 7–18 years. Depending on children’s age and reading competence, each questionnaire takes about 8–10 min for completion. Participants responded on a 4-point Likert scale (0, “never” to 3, “always”) to rate how frequently each statement applied to their daily cognitions, emotions and behaviours during the past two weeks. The BYI-II is designed for a large age range, as it operationalizes symptoms in easy language. That also facilitates the use of the BYI-II for immigrant groups, whose German language skills are often on average lower. In accordance with our study aims, the BYI-II questionnaires on depression, anxiety and self-concept were used in this study. We rigorously adhered to the BYI-II manual for scoring and missing value handling.

Self-concept

The positively worded self-concept inventory (Beck Self-Concept Inventory for Youth, BSCI-Y) assesses cognitive and emotional perceptions of competency and self-worth. It comprises the overall system of self-directed perceptions, cognitive and affective attitudes toward one's own person such as the perceived social role (e.g., "People want to be with me"), the personal strengths (e.g., "I am good at telling jokes") and self-acceptance (e.g., " I like myself "). The inventory showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α for immigrants = 0.86; Cronbach’s α for non-immigrants = 0.86).

Depression

The depression inventory (Beck Depression Inventory for Youth, BDI-Y) screens for symptoms of depression in line with the established criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, Fourth Edition [50]. This included low mood (e.g., "I feel sad"), somatic complaints (e.g., "I have trouble sleeping”) and latent suicidal tendencies (e.g., "I wish I were dead"). The inventory showed excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α for immigrants = 0.93; Cronbach’s α for non-immigrants = 0.93).

Anxiety

The anxiety inventory (Beck Anxiety Inventory for Youth, BAI-Y) screens for core symptoms of anxiety disorders relevant to children and adolescents such as general anxiety disorder, panic disorder and social phobia. In line with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, Fourth Edition [49], items reflect participants’ specific fears including in different situations (e.g., “I worry when I am at school”), worried social cognitions (e.g., “I worry people might get mad at me”) and physiological symptoms (e.g., “I get shaky”). The inventory showed very good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α for immigrants = 0.89; Cronbach’s α for non-immigrants = 0.91).

Data analyses

We performed data preparations and moderation models using SPSS version 25 [51] and regression models using R Version 3.5.0 [52]. Alpha-error probability was p < 0.05 (two-tailed) in all analyses. We adjusted for multiple comparisons using the “Multiple Comparisons Calculator” [53] in accordance with the procedure established by Bonferroni and Holm [54]. All p-values, considered in hypothesis testing are displayed in bold in the study tables.  We indicate significance only for those p-values after adjustment but consistently report unadjusted p-values throughout all tables for coherence in the illustration. For hypotheses on mediation, we interpret bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs). In preliminary analyses, we inspected data and checked statistical assumptions for parametric statistical procedures and computed a correlation matrix on variables considered in main analyses.

Hypotheses 1 and 2

For H1, we compared levels of depression, anxiety and self-concept between immigrant and non-immigrant youth while controlling for the covariates age, sex and parental socio-economic status. We therefore calculated two-step hierarchical regression models and, additionally, calculated R2 to determine the effect sizes. For H2, we similarly examined the impact of the self-concept on depression and anxiety, also controlling for age, sex and parental socio-economic status.

Hypotheses 3 and 4

We used second stage moderated mediation models [55] to test H3 and H4 as theoretically illustrated in Fig. 1a. We performed modeling using PROCESS, third version, a macro for SPSS developed by Hayes [56] that follows a regression-based approach to integrate mediation and moderation in a conditional process analysis [56]. As displayed in Fig. 1b, and contrary to the widely used historical method by Baron and Kenny [57], the regression-based approach quantifies the indirect effect between the antecedent X, the mediator M and the consequent Y itself and does not require an association between X and Y as a precondition [56]. The second stage moderated mediation model uses bootstrapping (k = 5000) to obtain estimates for 95% bias-corrected CIs for the conditional indirect effects in moderated mediation. The indirect effect is calculated for different conditional values of the moderator variable (i.e., two age groups—youth below or above 14 years of age). Conditional indirect effects are statistically significant when zero is not included between the lower and upper bound of the 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals generated for different values of the moderator variable W. The bootstrapping method is also superior to the Sobel test [58] due to higher statistical power and independency of the normal distribution of the underlying data [56].

Results

Descriptive analysis of the study sample

Of the N = 1907 children and adolescents, two did not state their age, another 66 did not complete the demographic section on the immigrant status of their parents or grandparents and were therefore excluded a-priori. This resulted in an effective study sample of N = 1839. About half of the participants were female (n = 49.8%) and participants were on average 14.05 years old (SD = 3.03). A large proportion of the sample comprised immigrant children and adolescents (n = 782, 42.5%) with most of these being in the second immigrant generation. Immigrant youth themselves or their families originated from 69 different countries with Turkey (n = 194, 10.5%), Poland (n = 111, 6.0%), and Russia (n = 42, 2.3%) being the most frequently stated countries. Immigrant youth overall came from families with a lower socio-economic status compared to non-immigrant youth. Given the population-based data collection procedure and demographic characteristics of the obtained study population, the immigrant group in our sample reflects the general population with immigrant backgrounds in Germany [47]. The measurements used in this study were created for an age range of 7–18 years, reflecting the schooling period in Germany. Seventy-six participants were outside of the defined age range, that is, 8 participants younger than 7 years and 68 participants older than 18 years. Since these students were members of the classrooms and their exclusion did not yield different results, they remained part of the presented analyses. Note that our effective study sample includes missing values of less than 0.9 percent per analyzed symptom variable. Due to the overall large sample size, we decided to conduct analyses based on that data. Table 1 reports basic socio-demographic information of the effective study sample. Table 2 gives an overview on the symptoms of depression and anxiety and the self-concept of the effective study sample. Table 3 shows correlations between study measures and socio-demographic information. 

Table 1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample
Table 2 Descriptive analysis of the Beck Youth Inventories (second edition) for symptoms of depression and anxiety by migration status and age
Table 3 Correlation matrix of depression, anxiety, self-concept, age, sex, SES and migration status

Examination of Hypotheses 1 and 2

In analyses for H1, the covariates sex and parental socio-economic status predicted symptom levels of depression and anxiety while in regressing self-concept the covariate age likely had additional predictive value. As expected in H1, immigrant children and adolescents reported increased symptom levels of depression (BUnstandardized = 1.223, adjusted p < 0.05) and anxiety (BUnstandardized = 2.179, adjusted p < 0.01) but, contrary to our expectations, did not report a lower self-concept compared to non-immigrants (BUnstandardized = 0.195, adjusted p > 0.05). Beyond the covariates, the migration status explained small additional proportions of unique variance in symptoms of anxiety (ΔR2 = 0.013) and depression (ΔR2 = 0.004; see Table 4). In analysis for H2, we found the self-concept to influence symptoms of depression such that a lower self-concept was linked to increased symptom levels (BUnstandardized = − 0.477, adjusted p < 0.001). Consistently, self-concept also predicted increased levels of anxiety in the same direction (BUnstandardized = − 0.283, adjusted p < 0.001). While the influence of the self-concept on both outcomes was substantial, it was larger on symptom levels of depression (ΔR2 = 0.183) than of anxiety (ΔR2 = 0.063). Details of the stepwise hierarchical regression analyses on H2 are presented in Table 5.

Table 4 Hierarchical regression analysis for migration status predicting symptoms of depression, anxiety and self-concept
Table 5 Hierarchical regression analysis for self-concept predicting symptoms of depression and anxiety

Moderated mediation analyses with the outcome depression (Hypotheses 3 and 4)

We predicted that age would moderate the indirect effect of the migration status on depression via self-concept (see Fig. 1a). The migration status did not influence the self-concept (path a). We consistently did not substantiate an overall indirect effect based on bootstrap confidence intervals (i.e., reduction of predictive value from path c to path c’). More specifically, we found no indirect effects of the focal predictor migration status on symptoms of depression for both age groups (children: indirect effect = − 0.016, 95% CI: − 0.358, 0.350; adolescents: indirect effect = − 0.020, 95% CI: − 0.485, 0.439). Self-concept thus did not represent a mediator for the demonstrated link between migration status and symptoms of depression. These findings contradicted the expectations of H3. However, the effect of self-concept on symptoms of depression depended on age as evidenced by the statistically significant interaction between M and W (path b3). While there was a negative link between self-concept and symptoms of depression in both age groups, this effect was stronger in the older group (children: ß = − 0.450, p < 0.001; adolescents: ß = − 0.587, p < 0.001). These findings were in line with H4 as a lower self-concept had a higher impact on increased symptom levels of depression during adolescence (i.e., participants older than 14 years of age). For further details see Table 6.

Table 6 Model coefficients for the moderated mediation models predicting symptoms of depression or anxiety

Moderated mediation analyses with the outcome anxiety (Hypotheses 3 and 4)

We predicted that age would moderate the indirect effect of migration status on symptoms of anxiety via self-concept (see Fig. 1a). The migration status did not affect the self-concept (path a). The indirect effect as estimated by bootstrap confidence intervals did not yield an effect. Specifically, we found no indirect influence of the focal predictor migration status on symptoms of anxiety for both age groups (children: indirect effect = − 0.009, 95% CI: − 0.198, 0.193; adolescents: indirect effect = − 0.411, 95% CI: − 0.345, 0.307). Self-concept thus did not function as a mediator for the demonstrated link between migration status and symptoms of anxiety. These findings similarly contradicted the expectations of H3. However, the effect of self-concept on symptoms of anxiety depended on age, as evidenced by the statistically significant interaction between M and W (path b3). While there was an overall negative link between self-concept and symptoms of anxiety, this link was consistently stronger in the older group (children: ß = − 0.242, p < 0.001; adolescents: ß = − 0.411, p < 0.001). These findings were in line with H4 as a lower self-concept had a higher impact on increased symptom levels of anxiety during adolescence. Overall, the moderated mediation model explained more variance of the outcome variable depression (R2 = 0.229) than of the outcome variable anxiety (R2 = 0.103). For further details see Table 6.

Discussion

This study integrated perspectives of developmental science and clinical child psychology to examine the self-concept as an intra-individual correlate of mental health disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant youth throughout youth development. We could support that immigrant youth report more symptoms of depression and anxiety than non-immigrant youth and could also show that a lower self-concept is linked to such symptoms. However, our analyses did not yield self-concept as a significant mediator for these associations. Despite this null effect we found that age moderated the association between self-concept and internalizing mental health problems. We discuss study evidence along the study hypotheses.

Immigrant children and adolescents experience more symptoms of depression and anxiety than non-immigrants

We hypothesized that having a migration background would link to increased levels of internalizing mental health problems. Indeed, migration status predicted symptoms of depression as well as anxiety. These findings contribute evidence to the ongoing debate whether migration status is linked to an increased risk for mental health problems (as hypothesized in this study), or alternatively facilitates processes of resilience. Contrary to our study propositions, some evidence on adolescent students in several European and other high-income countries substantiated the “immigrant (health) paradox”, that is, immigrant youth have a better mental health [59,60,61,62]. Based on a large dataset, our findings are however more consistent with our initial expectations of linking the migration status to lower mental health. Our evidence is supported by a stream of literature on the “mental health gap” between immigrant and non-immigrant youth [63, 64]. Possible explanations for the opposing propositions in the literature open two pathways. First, while many of the previous studies focus on immigrants from specific origin countries, our sample was more likely drawn from general immigrant populations in Germany including a variety of countries, ethno-cultural origins as well as immigrant generations. Through collecting data in non-exclusive classroom interviews, we did not limit our immigrant sample to specific groups. Our study sample thus more likely reflects the current and diverse population of children and adolescents with immigrant status in Germany. Some previous evidence suggests that the “immigrant health paradox” could encompass first-generation immigrants only [65]. Second, as our data was collected in school contexts, the identified mental health disparities might reflect consequences of social and ethnocultural discontinuities that immigrant children and adolescents can experience between home and school contexts [32].

Self-concept does not mediate the link between migration status and depression/anxiety

We found that a lower self-concept was overall linked to increased symptom levels of depression and anxiety. This finding is in line with our expectations and previous studies on children and adolescents from general populations [2830]. Based on the study sample, these findings moreover suggest that such associations could be generalizable to children and adolescents with diverse immigrant backgrounds.

We however did not find differences between immigrant and non-immigrant youth regarding their self-concepts. Correspondingly, our moderated mediation models did not substantiate a lower self-concept as a mediator for the link between migration status and internalizing mental health problems. These findings contradicted our expectations of a link between migration status and self-concept. At the same time, migration status significantly accounted for higher levels of internalizing mental health problems in our sample (H2). There are two approaches to explain such patterns of expected and unexpected findings. First, more specific dimensions of the self-concept, better reflecting a compartmentalized self-structure, rather than a global, unidimensional construct, merely reflecting an integrated self-structure, could better explain links between migration status and internalizing mental health problems. As a compartmentalized structure of the self was previously linked to lower mental health status and we substantiated fewer mental health problems for immigrant youth in our study, immigrant youth could have less accurately reported on their self-concepts using this global measure. Besides, the most relevant compartments of the self-concept that distinguish immigrant and non-immigrant youth may have not been in their cognitive foci when answering the self-concept measure. In a school context, compartments of the self-concept relating to academic components [66] likely influence cognitions when examining oneself. However, specific components of the self-concept, such as self-esteem or emotional stability, could have been relatively less salient for immigrant children and adolescents during study participation—even though such domains might be more strongly linked to internalizing mental health problems. Second, the self-concept, either in global or domain-specific operationalization, is not directly associated with the migration status as we hypothesized. Considering the deductions from the coexisting immigrant paradox, immigrant youth could be better off in some health-associated psychological domains (including the self-concept), while they simultaneously show increased symptom levels in others (including symptoms of depression and anxiety).

Age moderates the link between self-concept and depression/anxiety

Consistent with our expectations, we found that adolescents’ internalizing mental health problems are more strongly affected by a lower self-concept in our sample. The influence of the self-concept on the participants’ mental health status was moderated by age, meaning a lower self-concept seemed to impact adolescents more strongly than children. Considering age as a proxy for youth development, our evidence highlights the importance of developmental processes for understanding the relationship between self-concept and mental health. Consistent to this model and to our evidence, a previous study found older age and more internalizing mental health problems to predict lower self-concept in a multi-ethnic youth group in the United States [67].

Moreover, behavioral and cognitive covariations of the self-concept with children’s and adolescents’ age need additional consideration when interpreting our findings. For young children, the self-concept is less validated by outside cues and tends to be more positive in general [68]. Marsh, Parada, Yeung and Healey [69] postulated that younger children defend their self-concept with “troublemaking behavior” (i.e., socially directed strategies). While our study focus was on symptoms of depression and anxiety, the specific symptoms of mental health problems with either externalizing or internalizing nuances that are associated with a low self-concept could thus vary as a function of age. Another approach to explaining age differences in the self-concept focuses on its intrapersonal organization. In a study with youth suffering from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and internalizing mental problems [67], adolescents reported a lower self-concept than children. Study authors discussed whether such differences were either due to developmental processes influencing the organization of the self-concept or due to the stability of problem behavior which could have negatively affected the self-concept over time. Also, in our study, continuous social withdrawal could thus have limited the reception of environmental cues that are required for establishing a stable and positive self-concept during adolescence.

Strengths, limitations and future research

There were several limitations that underline the need for future research on the identification of within-person mediators for the mental health disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant youth. First of all, the sample was collected via self-report measures in school settings. To further validate our findings and substantiate generalization, we need replication studies with more sophisticated designs, such as using multi-informant approaches and longitudinal data collections from middle childhood to late adolescence. Second, the self-concept was measured via self-report questionnaires using a unidimensional construct applicable to a variety of social settings. The use of multi-dimensional measures, for example the Self-Description Questionnaire II [45], could better assess compartmentalized structures of the self-concept. Third, the BYI-II subscales were administered to a highly heterogenous group with immigrant backgrounds. To date, constructs as operationalized in these inventories still have to prove psychometric fidelity and measurement invariance, especially with regard to respondents’ ethnocultural backgrounds. Nonetheless, the present study has several strengths. It combined clinical child psychology and developmental science perspectives as it investigated the impact of the self-concept on symptoms of depression and anxiety from middle childhood to late adolescence. We included immigrant youth of the second- and third-generations from multiple origin countries to acknowledge youth’s diversity in current German society, while past research on adolescent mental health has extensively focused on first-generation immigrant populations [7, 20]. Regarding the study population, our evidence is based on a regionally and socio-economically controlled sample across the largest federal state in Germany and data was collected in a variety of different school types. However, we cannot fully preclude bias due to undetected mechanisms linked to socio-economic disparities. The parental socio-economic status of immigrant youth in our sample was on average lower in the immigrant sample. The parental socio-economic status was moreover linked to symptoms of anxiety which is consistent to previous findings.

Subsequent research needs to further investigate which within-person processes during childhood and adolescence mediate the link between migration status and internalizing mental health problems, possibly with regard to specific contexts of measurement. Beyond the self-concept, future studies could consider the children’s or their families’ cultural orientations including the significance of religion, or perceived discrimination at school. For example, Schunck, Reiss and Razum [70] provided evidence that perceived discrimination could mediate mental health disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant adults in Germany.

Implications

Our study findings overall underlined the importance of the self-concept for the mental health of children and adolescents. Our findings moreover emphasized that a positive self-concept is especially relevant during adolescence [71]. Although our study did not directly address the practical relevance of these findings, previous intervention studies repeatedly demonstrated that a strong self-concept has positive implications for youth’s mental health in school contexts. Rousseau et al. [72] evaluated a school-based prevention program that uses arts and self-expression to strengthen the self-concept of immigrant and multi-ethnic youth. Children enrolled in this program reported higher levels of well-being in comparison to a control group. Consistent evidence for addressing students at a younger age was provided by De Bettignies and Goldstein [73]: Improvisational theatre classes in elementary schools were found to improve children’s self-concept and mitigate internalizing mental health problems.

Conclusions

The proportion of immigrant children and adolescents in Germany has been increasing over recent years. Interdisciplinary practice and research integrating developmental science and clinical child psychology is required to not only consider highly diverse backgrounds among children and adolescents but also needs to acknowledge the fact that those from immigrant backgrounds have higher symptom levels of depression and anxiety. While our study supported such mental health disparities between immigrant and non-immigrant groups when assessed in school settings, data did not support that lower levels of the self-concept mediate these associations. Beyond replication studies to substantiate our findings, the search for underlying mediators needs to be continued with more specific research designs and additional focus on other migration-related candidate variables.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is available from the corresponding author and GS on reasonable request. Measures used for the primary outcomes can be obtained from the “Pearson Assessment & Information GmbH”.

Abbreviations

BAI-Y:

Beck Anxiety Inventory for Youth

BDI-Y:

Beck Depression Inventory for Youth

BSCI-Y:

Beck Self-Concept Inventory for Youth

BYI-II:

Beck Youth Inventory Second Edition

CIs:

Confidence intervals

ISCED:

International Standard Classification of Education

SES:

Socio-economic status

References

  1. Beck JS, Beck AT, Jolly JB. Beck youth inventories. 2nd ed. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 2005. p. 42.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Groen G, Petermann F. Depressive Kinder und Jugendliche. Berlin: Hogrefe; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Ravens-Sieberer U, Klasen F, Petermann F. Psychische Kindergesundheit. Kind und Entw. 2016;25:4–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1026/0942-5403/a000183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y. The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:56–64. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Carta MG, Bernal M, Hardoy MC, Haro-Abad JM. Migration and mental health in Europe (the state of the mental health in Europe working group: appendix 1). Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2005;1:13. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1745-0179-1-13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Belhadj Kouider E, Koglin U, Petermann F. Emotional and behavioral problems in migrant children and adolescents in Europe: a systematic review. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2014;23:373–91. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00787-013-0485-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Lustig SL, Kia-Keating M, Knight WG, Geltman P, Ellis H, Kinzie JD, Saxe GN. Review of child and adolescent refugee mental health. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:24–36. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/00004583-200401000-00012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Murad S, Joung I, Lenthe FJ, Bengi-Arslan L, Crijnen A. Predictors of self-reported problem behaviours in Turkish immigrant and Dutch adolescents in the Netherlands. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:412–23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/1469-7610.00131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Plener PL, Munz L, Allroggen M, Kapusta ND, Fegert JM, Groschwitz RC. Immigration as risk factor for non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts in adolescents in Germany. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:34. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13034-015-0065-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Van Oort FV, van der Ende J, Crijnen AA, Verhulst FC, Mackenbach JP, Joung IM. Ethnic disparities in problem behaviour in adolescence contribute to ethnic disparities in social class in adulthood. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2007;42:50–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Frank L, Yesil-Jürgens R, Born S, Hoffmann R, Santos-Hövener C, Lampert T. Maßnahmen zur verbesserten Einbindung und Beteiligung von Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Migrationshintergrund in KiGGS Welle 2. Journal of Health Monitoring. 2018;3:134–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.17886/RKI-GBE-2018-017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Stevens GWJM, Vollebergh AM. Mental health in migrant children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:276–94. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01848.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Szapocznik J, Kurtines WM. Family psychology and cultural diversity. Opportunities for theory, research and application. Am Psychol. 1993;48:400–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Sam DL, Virta E. Intergenerational value discrepancies in immigrant and host-national families and their impact on psychological adaptation. J Adolesc. 2003;26:213–31. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/s0140-1971(02)00129-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Buitelaar JK, Schulpen TWJ. Ethnic differences in parental detection of externalizing disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;15:418–26. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10802-007-9112-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zwirs BWC, Burger H, Schulpen TWJ, Buitelaar JK. Different treatment thresholds in non-Western children with behavioral problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45(4):476–83. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/01.chi.0000192251.46023.5a.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Park SY, Lee H, Kang M. Factors affecting health literacy among immigrants: systematic review. Eur J Public Health. 2018;28:406–7.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Babiker I, Cox J, Miller P. The measurement of cultural distance and its relationship to medical consultations: symptomatology and examination performance of overseas students at Edinburgh University. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1980;15:109–16. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/BF00578141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kashima ES, Abu-Rayya HM. Longitudinal associations of cultural distance with psychological well-being among Australian immigrants from 49 countries. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2014;45:587–600. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0022022113519857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gutmann M, Aysel M, Özlü-Erkilic Z, Popow C, Akkaya-Kalaycic T. Mental health problems of children and adolescents, with and without migration background, living in Vienna. Austria Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2019;13:35. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13034-019-0295-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Trautwein U, Köller O, Lüdtke O, Baumert J. Self-esteem, academic self-concept, and achievement: how the learning environment moderates the dynamics of the self-concept. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90:334–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Jekauc D, Wagner MO, Herrmann C, Hegazy K, Woll A. Does physical self-concept mediate the relationship between motor abilities and physical activity in adolescents and young adults? PLoS ONE. 2017;12:1. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0168539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Thomsen T, Lessing N, Greve W, Dresbach S. Selbstkonzept und Selbstwert. In: Lohaus A, editor. Entwicklung des Jugendalters. Berlin: Springer; 2018. p. 91–111.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  24. Showers CJ, Zeigler-Hill V. Pathways among self-knowledge and self-esteem: How are self-esteem and self-knowledge linked? Are these links direct or indirect? In: Kernis MH, editor. Self-esteem issues and answers: a source book of current perspectives. New York: Psychology Press; 2006. p. 216–23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0146167206294872.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  25. Showers CJ, Ditzfeld CP, Zeigler-Hill V. Self-concept structure and the quality of self-knowledge. J Pers. 2015;83(5):535–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/jopy.12130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Crocetti E, Rubini M, Luyckx K, Meeus W. Identity formation in early and middle adolescents from various ethnic groups: From three dimensions to five statuses. J Youth Adolesc. 2008;37:983–96. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10964-007-9222-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Möller, J & Trautwein, U. Selbstkonzept. In: Pädagogische Psychologie. Heidelberg: Springer, 2009.

  28. Emler N. Self esteem: the costs and causes of low self worth. New York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation, York Publishing Services Ltd; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Erkolahti R, Ilonen T. Saarijärvi S & Terho P Self-image and depressive symptoms among adolescents in a non-clinical sample. Nord J Psychiatry. 2003;57:447–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/08039480310003461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Räty LKA, Larsson G, Söderfelt BA, Wilde Larsson BH. Psychosocial aspects of health in adolescence: the influence of gender, and general self-concept. J Adolesc Health. 2005;36:521–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Martinsen KD, Neumer S-P, Holen S, Waaktaar T, Sund AM, Kendall PC. Self-reported quality of life and self-esteem in sad and anxious school children. BMC Psychol. 2016;4(1):45. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-016-0153-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Roebers CM, Schneider W. Self-concept and anxiety in immigrant children. Int J Beh Dev. 1999;23:125–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Bourque F, van der Ven E, Malla A. A meta-analysis of the risk for psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants. Psychol Med. 2011;41:897–910. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1017/S0033291710001406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Ceri V, Özlü-Erkilic Z, Özer Ü, Kadak T, Winkler D, Dogangün B, Akkaya-Kalayci T. Mental health problems of second generation children and adolescents with migration background. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2017;2:142–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/13651501.2017.1291820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Kizilhan JI. Potenziale und Belastungen psychosozialer Netzwerke in der Migration. In: Borde T, Davis M, editors. Migration und seelische Gesundheit, psychische Belastungen und Potenziale. Frankfurt: Mabuse; 2007. p. 53–68.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Odag-Wieacker I. Schwierigkeiten in der Identitätsbildung bei Kindern und Jugendlichen in Folge der Migration ihrer Elterngeneration. In: Burkhart-Mußmann C, Dammach B, editors. Migration, Flucht & Kindesentwicklung. Frankfurt: Brandes und Apsel; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Uslucan HH. Lebensweltliche Verunsicherung türkischer Migranten. Psychosozial. 2005;28:111–22.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Cole DA, Maxwell SE, Martin JM, Peeke LG, Seroczynski AD, Tram JM, Hoffmann KB, Ruiz MD, Jaquez F, Maschman T. The development of multiple domains of child and adolescent self-concept: a cohort sequential longitudinal design. Child Dev. 2001;72:1723–46. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/1467-8624.00375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Meeus W. The study of adolescent identity formation 2000–2010: A review of longitudinal research. J Res Adolesc. 2011;21:75–94. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00716.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Foulkes L, Blakemore S. Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence? Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;40:81–5. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Sherman LE, Payton AA, Hernandez LM, Greenfield PM, Dapretto M. The power of the like in adolescence: effects of peer influence on neural and behavioral responses to social media. Psychol Sci. 2016;27:1027–35. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0956797616645673.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Siefen RG. Psychische Entwicklungsrisiken bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Migrationshintergrund. In: Borde T, David M, editors. Kinder und Jugendliche mit Migrationshintergrund. Frankfurt: Mabuse; 2015. p. 107–19.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Schepker R. Jugendliche in der Migration. In: Machleidt W, Kluge U, Sieberer M, Heinz A, editors. Praxis der interkulturellen Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie. München: Elsevier; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Mitchell N. Academic achievement among caribbean immigrant adolescents: the impact of generational status on academic self-concept. Prof Sch Couns. 2005;8(3):209–18.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Marsh HW, Parada RH, Ayotte V. A Multidimensional perspective of relations between self-concept (Self-Description Questionnaire II) and adolescent mental health (Youth Self-Report). Psychol Assess. 2004;16:27–41. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ybrandt H. The relation between self-concept and social functioning in adolescence. J Adolesc. 2008;31:1–16. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Coelho VA, Marchante M, Jimerson SR. Promoting a positive middle school transition: a randomized-controlled treatment study examining self-concept and self-esteem. J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46:558–69. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10964-016-0510-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Ganzeboom HBG, De Graaf PM, Treiman DJ. A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Soc Sci Res. 1992;21:1–56. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/0049-089X(92)90017-B.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Siefen RG, Busch J. Beck youth inventories. 2nd ed. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  50. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: APA, 2000.

  51. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp., 2017.

  52. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Wien, Österreich: 2017. https://www.R-project.org/.

  53. Hemmerich, W. StatistikGuru: Rechner zur Adjustierung des α-Niveaus. 2016. Retrieved from https://statistikguru.de/rechner/adjustierung-des-alphaniveaus.html Accessed 29 Jan 2021

  54. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6:65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Edwards JR, Lambert LS. Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:1–22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. 2nd Edition. A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. JPSP. 1986;51:1173–82. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/0022-1031(82)90046-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol Methods Res. 1982;13:290–312. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2307/270723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Coll CG, Marks AK, editors. The immigrant paradox in children and adolescents: is becoming American a developmental risk? Washington: American Psychological Association; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Vaage AB, Tingvold L, Hauff E, Van Ta T, Wentzel-Larsen T, Clench-Aas J, Thomsen PH. Better mental health in children of Vietnamese refugees compared with their Norwegian peers: a matter of cultural difference? Child Adol Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3:34. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1753-2000-3-34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Markides K, Rote S. Immigrant health paradox. In: Scott RA, Kosslyn S, editors. Emerging trends in social and behavioral sciences. Hoboken: Wiley; 2015. p. 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Mood C, Jonsson JO, Låftman SB. Immigrant integration and youth mental health in four European countries. Eur Sociol Rev. 2016;32:716–29. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/ESR/JCW027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Dimitrova R, Chasiotis A, van de Vijver F. Adjustment outcomes of immigrant children and youth in Europe: a meta-analysis. Eur Psychol. 2016;21:150–62. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1027/1016-9040/a000246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Brettschneider A, Hölling H, Schlack R, Ellert U. Psychische Gesundheit von Jugendlichen in Deutschland. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2015;58:474–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Marks AK, Ejesi K, García CC. Understanding the US immigrant paradox in childhood and adolescence. Child Dev Perspect. 2014;8:59–64. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/cdep.12071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Krannich M, Goetz T, Lipnevich AA, Bieg M, Roos A, Becker ES, Morger V. Being over- or underchallenged in class: effects on students’ career aspirations via academic self-concept and boredom. Learn Individ Differ. 2019;69:206–18. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.10.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Houck G, Kendall J, Miller A, Morrell P, Wiebe G. Self concept in children and adolescents with ADHD. Pediatr Nurs. 2011;26:239–47. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.pedn.2010.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Marsh HW, Ellis LA, Craven RG. How do preschool children feel about themselves? Unraveling measurement and multidimensional self-concept structure. Dev Psychol. 2002;38:376–93. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0012-1649.38.3.376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Marsh HW, Parada RH, Yeung AS, Healey J. Aggressive school troublemakers and victims: a longitudinal model examining the pivotal role of self-concept. J Educ Psychol. 2001;93:411–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037//0022-0663.93.2.411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Schunck R, Reiss K, Razum O. Pathways between perceived discrimination and health among immigrants: evidence from a large national panel survey in Germany. Ethn Health. 2014;20:493–510. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/13557858.2014.932756.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Schulte-Körne G. Mental health problems in a school setting in children and adolescents. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2016;113(11):183–90. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3238/arztebl.2016.0183.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Rousseau C, Drapeau A, Lacroix L, Bagilishya D, Heusch N. Evaluation of a classroom program of creative expression workshops for refugee and immigrant children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(2):180–5. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00344.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. De Bettignies BH & Goldstein TR. Improvisational theater classes improve self-concept. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2019. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2019-29428-001. Accessed 31 Jan 2020.

  74. UNESCO Institute of Statistics. International Standard Classification of Education ISCED 2011. 2012. http://uis.unesco.org/sites/default/files/documents/international-standard-classification-of-education-isced-2011-en.pdf. Accessed 31 Jan 2020.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The investigators gratefully thank research assistants, Eva Krey, Jasmine Golembe and Cordula Behr, for their contributions to the research project, schools and communities who were key to recruitment in this study and, finally, participating children and adolescents for contributing their time and effort to support our research.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. Data collection of this study was supported by “Pearson Assessment & Information GmbH”. Publication of the study was supported by the DFG Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr-University Bochum. The funders had no active role in study design, data collection and analysis, interpretation of data, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JB contributed to the study by conceptualization, methodology, data interpretation, study coordination and by drafting and revising parts of the article. CC contributed to the study by conceptualization, methodology, data curation, data interpretation and by drafting parts of the article. SS contributed to the study by investigation, conceptualization and providing resources. GS contributed to the study by supervising the research project, funding acquisition, providing resources, revising drafts of the manuscript. All authors provided critical feedback on the manuscript and gave their approval for publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Busch.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Germany Psychological Society. All participating children and their parents gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The full study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology at Ruhr-University Bochum (2016-122).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Busch, J., Claus, C., Schneider, S. et al. Does a lower self-concept contribute to mental health disparities of diverse immigrant youth from middle childhood to late adolescence?. BMC Psychol 9, 59 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-021-00555-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40359-021-00555-0

Keywords